Abstract

Semantic memory refers to knowledge about people, objects, actions, relations, self, and culture acquired through experience. The neural systems that store and retrieve this information have been studied for many years, but a consensus regarding their identity has not been reached. Using strict inclusion criteria, we analyzed 120 functional neuroimaging studies focusing on semantic processing. Reliable areas of activation in these studies were identified using the activation likelihood estimate (ALE) technique. These activations formed a distinct, left-lateralized network comprised of 7 regions: posterior inferior parietal lobe, middle temporal gyrus, fusiform and parahippocampal gyri, dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, inferior frontal gyrus, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, and posterior cingulate gyrus. Secondary analyses showed specific subregions of this network associated with knowledge of actions, manipulable artifacts, abstract concepts, and concrete concepts. The cortical regions involved in semantic processing can be grouped into 3 broad categories: posterior multimodal and heteromodal association cortex, heteromodal prefrontal cortex, and medial limbic regions. The expansion of these regions in the human relative to the nonhuman primate brain may explain uniquely human capacities to use language productively, plan, solve problems, and create cultural and technological artifacts, all of which depend on the fluid and efficient retrieval and manipulation of semantic knowledge.

Introduction

The human brain has an enormous capacity to acquire knowledge from experience. The characteristic shapes, colors, textures, movements, sounds, smells, and actions associated with objects in the environment, for example, must all be learned from experience. Much of this knowledge is represented symbolically in language and underlies our understanding of word meanings. These relationships between words and the stores of knowledge they signify are known collectively as the “semantics” of a language (Bréal 1897). In this article, we use the more general term “semantic processing” to refer to the cognitive act of accessing stored knowledge about the world.

For most languages, semantic properties of words are readily distinguished from their structural properties. For example, words can have both spoken (phonological) and written (orthographic) forms, but these surface forms are typically related to word meanings only through the arbitrary conventions of a particular vocabulary. There is nothing, for example, about the letter sequences DOG or CHIEN that inherently links these sequences to a particular concept. Conversely, it is trivial to construct surface forms (e.g., CHOG) that possess all of the phonological and orthographic properties of words in a particular language, but which have no meaning in that language. In this article, we hold to a simple, operational distinction between 1) the processes of analyzing surface form (phonology, orthography), and 2) semantic processes, which concern access to knowledge not directly represented in the surface form.

Semantic processing is a defining feature of human behavior, central not only to language, but also to our capacity to access acquired knowledge in reasoning, planning, and problem solving. Impairments of semantic processing figure in a variety of brain disorders such as Alzheimer disease, semantic dementia, fluent aphasia, schizophrenia, and autism. The neural basis of semantic processing has been studied extensively by analyzing patterns of brain damage in such patients (e.g., Alexander et al. 1989; Hart and Gordon 1990; Chertkow et al. 1997; Tranel et al. 1997; Gainotti 2000; Mummery et al. 2000; Hillis et al. 2001; Damasio et al. 2004; Dronkers et al. 2004). On the whole, this evidence suggests a broadly distributed neural representation, with particular reliance on inferotemporal and posterior inferior parietal regions. Semantic processing has also been addressed in a large number of functional neuroimaging studies conducted on healthy volunteers, using positron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). The aim of the present study is to conduct a meta-analysis of this functional neuroimaging research, which now includes over 500 published studies. Several excellent reviews on neuroimaging studies of semantic processing have been presented previously, which focused mainly on the evidence for organization of object knowledge by taxonomic categories (Joseph 2001; Martin and Chao 2001; Bookheimer 2002; Thompson-Schill 2003; Damasio et al. 2004; Gerlach 2007). The present analysis differs from these prior efforts in including a larger number and broader range of studies, in adopting specific inclusion and exclusion criteria for identifying semantic processing experiments, and in the construction of probabilistic maps for summarizing the data.

Considered for inclusion were any PET or fMRI studies in which words (either spoken or written) were used as stimulus materials. Thus, the goal of the current study is to identify brain systems that access meaning from words. This approach contrasts with several previous reviews that included and even emphasized studies in which object pictures were used to elicit knowledge retrieval (Joseph 2001; Martin and Chao 2001; Damasio et al. 2004; Gerlach 2007). Our focus on linguistic materials reflects our present concern, which is not how objects are recognized, but rather how conceptual knowledge is organized and accessed. Although the knowledge stores underlying word comprehension may be activated similarly during word and object recognition tasks, there is also evidence that these 2 semantic access routes are not identical. For example, object recognition engages a complex, hierarchical perceptual stream that encodes progressively more abstract representations of object features and their spatial relationships (Marr 1982; Tanaka et al. 1991; Logothetis and Sheinberg 1996; Humphreys and Forde 2001). Certainly not all of these perceptual representations are encoded in language or even available to awareness. At present, it is not clear that comprehension of a word necessarily entails activation of a detailed perceptual representation of the object to which it refers, at least not to the same degree as that evoked by the object itself. In fact, many functional neuroimaging studies suggest different patterns of activation during matched word and picture recognition tasks (Gorno-Tempini et al. 1998; Moore and Price 1999b; Chee et al. 2000; Hasson et al. 2002; Bright et al. 2004; Gates and Yoon 2005; Reinholz and Pollmann 2005). The existence of patients with profound visual object recognition disorders but relatively intact word comprehension also argues against a complete overlap between the knowledge systems underlying word and object recognition (Warrington 1985; Lissauer 1889; Farah 1990; Davidoff and De Bleser 1994). In the interest of maintaining a clear focus on the processing of concepts rather than percepts, we thus elected to include in this analysis only those experiments that used words as stimuli.

Specific exclusion criteria are a critical feature of the present study. The numerous published neuroimaging studies concerning semantic processing address a variety of specific topics and employ a wide range of tasks and task comparisons (here referred to as “contrasts”). The authors of these studies often began from very different theoretical perspectives and varied in their interpretation of task demands. One approach to selecting material for a meta-analysis would have been to include any study considered by the original authors to have addressed semantic processing, as indicated, for example, by the study title or list of keywords. We found during initial attempts to review these reports, however, that markedly different, largely nonoverlapping activation patterns were frequently observed across studies. Furthermore, this discordance was related mainly to variability in task selection and the type of task contrast used. Three problematic features of this literature were of particular interest and were found with some regularity. First, many studies employed contrasting tasks that differed on phonological or orthographic processing demands in addition to semantic processing demands (see examples in Table 1). It is a general principle of functional neuroimaging studies, and one that warrants strong emphasis, that the “activations” measured using these methods are in fact representations of the relative differences in neural activity between 2 or more brain states. Thus, the pattern of activated brain regions observed in a study putatively targeting semantic processes depends not only on the cognitive processes elicited by the semantic task, but also on the processes elicited, or not elicited, by the comparison task. If the comparison task does not make demands on phonological and orthographic processes that approximate those made by the semantic task (as is the case, e.g., with any control task involving unpronounceable or unnameable stimuli), then the resulting activation is as likely to reflect phonological or orthographic processes as semantic processes. Such contrasts were excluded from the present analysis.

Table 1

Example contrasts in which the semantic and control conditions are not matched on orthographic or phonological processing demands

Semantic conditionControl condition
Stimulus, for exampleTaskStimulus, for exampleTask
horseRead silently/ / / / /View silently
horseRead aloudυβγσφSay “OK”
horseRead silentlynbgsjRead silently
horseDoes it have an ascender?nbgsjDoes it have an ascender?
horseIs it living or nonliving?nbgsjDoes it have more than 4 letters?
horse, zebraAre they related in meaning?nbgsj, nbqsjAre they identical?
“horse”Listendistorted speechListen
“horse”Is it living or nonliving?tonesIs it high or low in pitch?
“horse”Is it living or nonliving?distorted speechIs it a male or female voice?
“horse”Is it living or nonliving?“ba”Is it “ba” or “pa”?
Semantic conditionControl condition
Stimulus, for exampleTaskStimulus, for exampleTask
horseRead silently/ / / / /View silently
horseRead aloudυβγσφSay “OK”
horseRead silentlynbgsjRead silently
horseDoes it have an ascender?nbgsjDoes it have an ascender?
horseIs it living or nonliving?nbgsjDoes it have more than 4 letters?
horse, zebraAre they related in meaning?nbgsj, nbqsjAre they identical?
“horse”Listendistorted speechListen
“horse”Is it living or nonliving?tonesIs it high or low in pitch?
“horse”Is it living or nonliving?distorted speechIs it a male or female voice?
“horse”Is it living or nonliving?“ba”Is it “ba” or “pa”?

Note: The “living/nonliving” task is intended to represent a variety of similar semantic decision tasks. All examples are from the studies reviewed.

Table 1

Example contrasts in which the semantic and control conditions are not matched on orthographic or phonological processing demands

Semantic conditionControl condition
Stimulus, for exampleTaskStimulus, for exampleTask
horseRead silently/ / / / /View silently
horseRead aloudυβγσφSay “OK”
horseRead silentlynbgsjRead silently
horseDoes it have an ascender?nbgsjDoes it have an ascender?
horseIs it living or nonliving?nbgsjDoes it have more than 4 letters?
horse, zebraAre they related in meaning?nbgsj, nbqsjAre they identical?
“horse”Listendistorted speechListen
“horse”Is it living or nonliving?tonesIs it high or low in pitch?
“horse”Is it living or nonliving?distorted speechIs it a male or female voice?
“horse”Is it living or nonliving?“ba”Is it “ba” or “pa”?
Semantic conditionControl condition
Stimulus, for exampleTaskStimulus, for exampleTask
horseRead silently/ / / / /View silently
horseRead aloudυβγσφSay “OK”
horseRead silentlynbgsjRead silently
horseDoes it have an ascender?nbgsjDoes it have an ascender?
horseIs it living or nonliving?nbgsjDoes it have more than 4 letters?
horse, zebraAre they related in meaning?nbgsj, nbqsjAre they identical?
“horse”Listendistorted speechListen
“horse”Is it living or nonliving?tonesIs it high or low in pitch?
“horse”Is it living or nonliving?distorted speechIs it a male or female voice?
“horse”Is it living or nonliving?“ba”Is it “ba” or “pa”?

Note: The “living/nonliving” task is intended to represent a variety of similar semantic decision tasks. All examples are from the studies reviewed.

A second, more prevalent problem concerns contrasts in which the conditions differed in task difficulty. Functional neuroimaging measurements are very sensitive to differences in response time, accuracy, and level of effort between tasks (see, e.g., Braver et al. 1997; Jonides et al. 1997; Honey et al. 2000; Adler et al. 2001; Braver et al. 2001; Ullsperger and von Cramon 2001; Gould et al. 2003; Binder et al. 2004; Binder, Medler, et al. 2005; Mitchell 2005; Sabsevitz et al. 2005; Desai et al. 2006; Lehmann et al. 2006; Tregallas et al. 2006). Such differences pose a problem if there are cognitive systems supporting general task performance functions that are modulated by task difficulty. Likely examples of such domain-general systems include a sustained attention network for maintaining arousal, a selective attention system for focusing neural resources on a particular modality or sensory object in the environment (e.g., a visual display), a working memory system for keeping task instructions and task-relevant sensory representations accessible, a response selection mechanism for mapping the contents of working memory to a response, a response inhibition system for preventing premature or prepotent responses from being made in error, and an error-monitoring system for adjusting response criteria and response time deadlines to minimize such errors. These systems, located mainly in frontal, anterior cingulate, and dorsal parietal cortices (Corbetta et al. 1998; Carter et al. 1999; Duncan and Owen 2000; Grosbras et al. 2005; Owen et al. 2005), are necessary for completing any task. If this is the case, and if the level of activation in these systems depends on general task demands, then it follows that activation can never be attributed with certainty to semantic processing when this activation has resulted from a contrast in which general task demands differ. Such contrasts (see examples in Table 2) were therefore excluded from the present analysis.

Table 2

Examples for which the contrasted conditions are not matched on general task difficulty due to task set, word frequency, ambiguity, or priming effects

Hard word conditionEasy word condition
Stimulus, for exampleTaskStimulus, for exampleTask
horseGenerate associated wordhorseRead
Generate names of animalsGenerate months of the year
horseIs it living or nonliving?horseRead silently
impalaIs it living or nonliving?horseIs it living or nonliving?
horseIs it living or nonliving?horse (repeated)Is it living or nonliving?
horseIs it living or nonliving?zebraDoes it contain the letter ‘a'?
horse, zebraAre they related in meaning?horse, horseAre they identical?
house, zebraAre they related in meaning?horse, horseAre they the same font size?
house, zebraAre they related in meaning?horse, ZEBRAAre they the same case?
bank, riverAre they related in meaning?lake, riverAre they related in meaning?
house, zebraIs the second item a word?horse, zebraIs the second item a word?
Hard word conditionEasy word condition
Stimulus, for exampleTaskStimulus, for exampleTask
horseGenerate associated wordhorseRead
Generate names of animalsGenerate months of the year
horseIs it living or nonliving?horseRead silently
impalaIs it living or nonliving?horseIs it living or nonliving?
horseIs it living or nonliving?horse (repeated)Is it living or nonliving?
horseIs it living or nonliving?zebraDoes it contain the letter ‘a'?
horse, zebraAre they related in meaning?horse, horseAre they identical?
house, zebraAre they related in meaning?horse, horseAre they the same font size?
house, zebraAre they related in meaning?horse, ZEBRAAre they the same case?
bank, riverAre they related in meaning?lake, riverAre they related in meaning?
house, zebraIs the second item a word?horse, zebraIs the second item a word?

Note: All examples are from the studies reviewed.

Table 2

Examples for which the contrasted conditions are not matched on general task difficulty due to task set, word frequency, ambiguity, or priming effects

Hard word conditionEasy word condition
Stimulus, for exampleTaskStimulus, for exampleTask
horseGenerate associated wordhorseRead
Generate names of animalsGenerate months of the year
horseIs it living or nonliving?horseRead silently
impalaIs it living or nonliving?horseIs it living or nonliving?
horseIs it living or nonliving?horse (repeated)Is it living or nonliving?
horseIs it living or nonliving?zebraDoes it contain the letter ‘a'?
horse, zebraAre they related in meaning?horse, horseAre they identical?
house, zebraAre they related in meaning?horse, horseAre they the same font size?
house, zebraAre they related in meaning?horse, ZEBRAAre they the same case?
bank, riverAre they related in meaning?lake, riverAre they related in meaning?
house, zebraIs the second item a word?horse, zebraIs the second item a word?
Hard word conditionEasy word condition
Stimulus, for exampleTaskStimulus, for exampleTask
horseGenerate associated wordhorseRead
Generate names of animalsGenerate months of the year
horseIs it living or nonliving?horseRead silently
impalaIs it living or nonliving?horseIs it living or nonliving?
horseIs it living or nonliving?horse (repeated)Is it living or nonliving?
horseIs it living or nonliving?zebraDoes it contain the letter ‘a'?
horse, zebraAre they related in meaning?horse, horseAre they identical?
house, zebraAre they related in meaning?horse, horseAre they the same font size?
house, zebraAre they related in meaning?horse, ZEBRAAre they the same case?
bank, riverAre they related in meaning?lake, riverAre they related in meaning?
house, zebraIs the second item a word?horse, zebraIs the second item a word?

Note: All examples are from the studies reviewed.

A final issue concerns the interpretation of states in which no overt task is required. Many neuroimaging studies used states described as “passive” or “resting” in which subjects are given either no task, a minimally demanding task such as fixating a point in the visual field, or a nominal task for which compliance is uncertain and unknowable (e.g., “clear your mind,” “focus on the scanner sounds”). Although such conditions can be useful as a low-level baseline, particularly for sensory stimulation studies, their use in semantic studies is problematic. Most people report experiencing vivid and memorable thoughts and mental images during such conscious, attentive states (James 1890; Antrobus et al. 1966; Pope and Singer 1976; Singer 1993; Giambra 1995; Binder et al. 1999; McKiernan et al. 2006). We argued previously that the processes supporting such “task-unrelated thoughts” are essentially semantic, because they depend on activation and manipulation of acquired knowledge about the world (Binder et al. 1999; McKiernan et al. 2006). If such states involve semantic processing, their use as a baseline runs the risk of subtracting away any semantic processing elicited by an overt semantic task. Such a contrast (an active semantic task compared with no task) also violates the stipulations, discussed above, that task conditions be matched on low-level processing demands and on overall difficulty, and would thus be expected to produce “false positive” activation of surface form processing and general executive systems. Such contrasts were therefore excluded from the present analysis. Also excluded were contrasts in which 2 conditions that both involve passive stimulation were compared, such as passively listening to words versus pseudowords. In these cases, the processes underlying generation of task-unrelated thoughts, which we consider to be largely semantic in nature, would predominate in both of the conditions being contrasted, resulting in little or no relative activation of semantic systems.

In summary, the present meta-analysis represents a critical review in which selection criteria are based on specific theoretical distinctions between semantic and surface form processing, and between semantic processes and more general processes required for task execution. Our aim is thus to identify brain regions that contribute specifically to the semantic component of word recognition, that is, the activation of stored conceptual knowledge, apart from the accompanying analysis of surface form or generation of an overt task response.

Materials and Methods

Study Identification

Procedures for identifying candidate studies were designed to be as inclusive as possible. Candidate studies were identified through searches of the PubMed, Medline, and PsychINFO online databases for the years 1980 through 2007. This search was conducted using the following Boolean operation applied to title, abstract, and keyword fields: <“brain mapping” OR “functional magnetic resonance imaging” OR “fMRI” OR “positron emission tomography” OR “PET” OR “neuroimaging”> AND <“semantics” OR “semantic memory” OR “category” OR “conceptual knowledge”>. This step yielded 2832 unique items. Abstracts from these studies were then initially screened to identify those that used either fMRI or PET and included healthy human participants, yielding 790 articles. Abstracts from these articles were then evaluated in more depth to identify experiments that used word stimuli and included either a general or specific semantic contrast. If this information could not be determined from the abstract, the article was also included. This second screening step yielded 431 articles. Online tables of contents and abstracts for selected journals focusing on cognition and neuroimaging, including “Brain and Language,” “Human Brain Mapping,” “Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,” and “NeuroImage,” and previously published reviews of semantic neuroimaging studies (Cabeza and Nyberg 2000; Joseph 2001; Martin and Chao 2001; Bookheimer 2002; Devlin, Russell, et al. 2002; Price and Friston 2002; Martin and Caramazza 2003; Thompson-Schill 2003; Damasio et al. 2004; Vigneau et al. 2006; Gerlach 2007) were then manually searched back to 1995 for relevant articles, yielding an additional 72 items. Finally, any additional relevant articles known to the authors, cited in the initial set of articles, or encountered during the review process were added to the list, resulting in a total of 520 articles that underwent full review.

The full review process included a complete reading of each article by 1 of the 4 authors, followed by application of the following inclusion criteria:In an effort to be as inclusive as possible, criteria 6 and 7 were not applied in a rigid manner. For example, studies that did not include adequate documentation of task performance were included if the reviewer judged the tasks to be approximately equal in difficulty. Studies were also included if the control task was more difficult than the semantic task, following the logic that relative activation during the easier semantic task could not in such cases be due to greater demands on general executive processes. Criterion 7 was applied to avoid sampling bias for particular brain regions, but studies were included if nearly all of the cerebrum was imaged. Cases in which adherence to criteria was ambiguous were reviewed by a second author to reach a consensus view.

  1. fMRI or PET study involving healthy adult human participants,

  2. use of spoken or written word stimuli,

  3. use of one or more semantic contrasts (see definition below),

  4. incorporation of controls for sensory and word-form (orthographic and phonological) processes,

  5. incorporation of controls for general executive and response processes,

  6. use of a control task with overall difficulty at least as great as the semantic task,

  7. image data acquired over all or most of the supratentorial brain,

  8. availability of peak activation coordinates from a group activation map.

All studies included after full review by one of the authors were then reviewed by a second author to confirm eligibility. Rare disagreements were resolved by further discussion among the authors.

Types of Semantic Contrasts

Two types of contrasts are relevant to the goal of identifying activation due to semantic processing. The first, which we refer to as a “general semantics” contrast, follows from the operational distinction between word structure and meaning discussed above. A general contrast is one between a condition that elicits high levels of access to word meaning and a condition that elicits lower levels of access to word meaning. The contrast must include controls for processing word structure (phonology and orthography) as well as for general executive and response processes. The 3 most common general contrasts were:

  1. Words versus Pseudowords. Pseudowords are spoken or written stimuli with structural properties similar to real words. The task performed on the words and pseudowords is nominally equivalent (e.g., an orthographic or phonological matching task, reading aloud, or lexical decision), such that the main difference emphasized by the contrast is the additional access to meaning in the word condition.

  2. Semantic Task versus Phonological Task. This contrast involves a comparison between different tasks, one of which focuses attention on semantic aspects of the stimuli (e.g., a semantic decision task) and the other on structural (usually phonological) attributes of the stimuli (e.g., a rhyme decision or phoneme detection task). Typically, all the stimuli are words, thus the contrast emphasizes the additional access to word meaning elicited by the task in the semantic condition. Combinations of contrasts 1) and 2) also occur, in which a semantic task involving words is compared with a phonological task involving pseudowords (e.g., Démonet et al. 1992; Cappa et al. 1998; Binder et al. 1999).

  3. High versus Low Meaningfulness. This contrast is a variation on 1), in which the contrasting stimuli differ in meaningfulness but are not words and pseudowords. Some examples include names of famous versus unknown people, related versus unrelated word pairs, and meaningful versus nonsensical sentences.

The other type of semantic contrast, which we designate “specific,” entails a comparison between hypothetically distinct types of conceptual knowledge. The aim of such studies is not to delineate the entire semantic processing system, but rather to identify putative functional subdivisions within the semantic system. Many such studies, for example, involve comparisons between concrete objects from different categories (e.g., animals vs. tools). Such studies are relevant to our aims because they contribute to the identification of brain regions involved specifically in semantic processing. Because we are not concerned here with particular functional subdivisions within the semantic system, activation data from both sides of the contrast (e.g., activation for animals > tools and for tools > animals) were included. As with the general contrasts, several variations can be distinguished based on whether the contrast pertains to stimulus or task manipulations. For example, in experiments involving a stimulus manipulation, a constant task is used (usually a semantic decision) with 2 (or more) contrasting categories of stimuli. In experiments involving a task manipulation, the attentional focus of the participant is switched to different semantic attributes (e.g., color, action, and size) of the same concepts using changes in task set.

We report here results obtained from separate analyses of the general and specific contrasts as well as a global analysis combining all studies. The specific foci were classified according to type of semantic knowledge represented by each contrast, and separate analyses were conducted for each specific knowledge type for which sufficient data were available.

Data Analysis

For each study reviewed, all reported contrasts that met inclusion criteria were included in the analysis. For each such contrast, the reviewer recorded the number of participants contributing to the activation map, the imaging modality used, the type of contrast, a brief description of the stimuli and tasks used in the contrast, the standard space to which the data were normalized, all reported coordinate locations for activation peaks, the Z values associated with each peak (if available), and the published table from which the coordinates were copied.

All coordinates were converted to a single common stereotaxic space. The studies were evenly divided between those that reported coordinates in the standard space of Talairach and Tournoux (1988) and those that used a variation of MNI space (Evans et al. 1993). We converted all MNI coordinates to the Talairach and Tournoux (1988) system using the “icbm2tal” transform (Lancaster et al. 2007). This transform reduces the bias associated with reference frame and scale in MNI–Talairach conversion.

Probabilistic maps of the resulting sets of coordinates were constructed using the “Activation Likelihood Estimate” (ALE) method (Turkeltaub et al. 2002), implemented in the GingerALE software package (Laird et al. 2005) (available at www.brainmap.org), using an 8-mm FWHM 3D Gaussian point spread function and a spatial grid composed of 2 × 2 × 2 mm voxels. This method treats each reported focus as the center of a Gaussian probability distribution. The 3D Gaussian distributions corresponding to all foci included in a given analysis are summed to create a whole-brain map that represents the overlap of activation peaks at each voxel, referred to as the ALE statistic. Subsequent analysis is restricted to a brain volume mask (Kochunov et al. 2002) distributed with the GingerALE software. To determine the null distribution of the ALE statistic for each analysis, a Monte Carlo simulation with 10 000 iterations was performed, in which each iteration consisted of a set of foci equal in number to the observed data, placed at random locations within the analysis volume and convolved with the same point spread function (Laird et al. 2005). Based on these null distributions, the ALE statistic maps for each analysis are converted to voxelwise probability maps.

The probability of chance formation of suprathreshold clusters was then determined by Monte Carlo simulation using in-house software, with 1000 iterations. Each iteration contained randomly located foci equal in number to the observed data and convolved with the same point spread function. The ALE map was then computed for each iteration, and the number and size of voxel clusters were recorded after thresholding each simulated data set at various voxelwise ALE thresholds. ALE maps from each of the observed data sets were then thresholded at an ALE value that yielded a corrected mapwise α < 0.05 after removing clusters smaller than 1000 μL. For visualization of probability values at each voxel, these corrected ALE maps were applied as masks on the probability maps generated by the GingerALE software. These thresholded probability maps are shown in Figs. 3–6.

Results

The initial search and screening procedures identified 520 articles, which were subsequently reviewed in detail. A total of 120 articles met inclusion criteria and provided 187 semantic contrasts. Figure 1 shows a breakdown of these studies by year published. A list of the included studies is provided in  Appendix 1. Six of the included studies (Paulesu et al. 2000; Giraud and Price 2001; Tyler et al. 2001; Devlin, Russell, et al. 2002; Pilgrim et al. 2002; Liu et al. 2006) met all criteria, but no activation was observed for the semantic contrasts of interest. Among the 400 excluded studies, 10 used techniques other than fMRI or PET, or studied special subject populations. Six were reviews or reanalyses of previously published data. About 20% (82) of the excluded studies did not include any semantic contrasts, 13% (52) did not use word stimuli, 9% (37) used a resting or passive condition as the only control, 31% (127) used active control tasks that were less difficult than the semantic task, and 16% (66) used active control tasks that did not control for word-form (orthographic or phonological) processing. In 17% (69) of the excluded studies, no group activation data were reported for the semantic contrast of interest, or foci were reported only for a priori regions of interest.

Distribution of the included studies by year published.
Figure 1.

Distribution of the included studies by year published.

Of the 187 contrasts that met all inclusion criteria, 87 were of the general type and 100 of the specific type; 126 used fMRI, and 61 used PET. These studies collectively involved 1642 participants and yielded 1145 activation foci (691 general and 454 specific). (The number of unique individuals involved in these studies could be smaller than 1642. We did not count participants more than once if they were involved in more than one contrast from the same publication. However, the same individual could have been included in more than one publication.)

All Semantic Contrasts

Figure 2 shows activation foci from all of the included studies projected onto an inflated surface model of the cerebral cortex (see Figs. 1 and 2 in the Supplemental Material online for similar images color coded by type of contrast). Of the 1145 published foci, 10 were located in the cerebellum and are therefore not shown. About 68% (771) of the foci were in the left cerebral hemisphere (x < 0) and 32% (362) in the right hemisphere (x > 0), indicating moderate left-hemisphere lateralization. Foci were located throughout the brain, but with strong clustering in some regions, particularly the left inferior parietal lobe. Areas with notably few foci included the precentral and postcentral gyri bilaterally, primary and secondary visual cortices, superior parietal lobules, frontal eye fields, and dorsal anterior cingulate gyri. There was a clear line of demarcation along the lateral bank of the left intraparietal sulcus, separating a large and dense cluster of foci in the inferior parietal lobe from uninvolved cortex in the intraparietal sulcus and superior parietal lobe.

One thousand one hundred and thirty-five published activation foci from the included studies projected onto an inflated cortical surface.
Figure 2.

One thousand one hundred and thirty-five published activation foci from the included studies projected onto an inflated cortical surface.

The thresholded ALE probability map based on all 1145 foci is shown in Figure 3. Activations were lateralized to the left hemisphere and widely distributed in frontal, temporal, parietal, and paralimbic areas. Seven principal regions showed a high likelihood of activation across studies: 1) the angular gyrus (AG) and adjacent supramarginal gyrus (SMG); 2) the lateral temporal lobe, including the entire length of the middle temporal gyrus (MTG) and posterior portions of the inferior temporal gyrus (ITG); 3) a ventromedial region of the temporal lobe centered on the mid-fusiform gyrus and adjacent parahippocampus; 4) dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (DMPFC) in the superior frontal gyrus (SFG) and adjacent middle frontal gyrus (MFG); 5) the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), especially the pars orbitalis; 6) ventromedial and orbital prefrontal cortex; and 7) the posterior cingulate gyrus and adjacent ventral precuneus. Weaker activations occurred at homologous locations in the right hemisphere, principally the AG, posterior MTG, and posterior cingulate gyrus. See Appendix 2 for details regarding activation of each of these regions in each of the included studies.

ALE map of all semantic foci, thresholded at whole-brain corrected P < 0.05. Activations are displayed on serial sagittal sections through the stereotaxic space of Talairach and Tournoux (1988) at 4-mm intervals, with slice locations given at the lower left of each image. Green lines indicate the stereotaxic y and z axes. Tick marks indicate 10-mm intervals. The color scale indicates voxelwise probability values of P < 0.01 (red), P < 0.001 (orange), and P < 0.0001 (yellow).
Figure 3.

ALE map of all semantic foci, thresholded at whole-brain corrected P < 0.05. Activations are displayed on serial sagittal sections through the stereotaxic space of Talairach and Tournoux (1988) at 4-mm intervals, with slice locations given at the lower left of each image. Green lines indicate the stereotaxic y and z axes. Tick marks indicate 10-mm intervals. The color scale indicates voxelwise probability values of P < 0.01 (red), P < 0.001 (orange), and P < 0.0001 (yellow).

General Contrasts

Figure 4 shows the thresholded ALE map for the 691 general semantic foci. This map is very similar to the one derived from all foci, though with generally smaller clusters. Activation in the IFG is more clearly localized to the pars orbitalis. Activation in the left fusiform gyrus extends somewhat farther anteriorly, and there is more extensive involvement of the left ventromedial prefrontal region.

ALE map of 691 foci resulting from general semantic contrasts (see Methods for details). Formatting and thresholding as in Figure 3.
Figure 4.

ALE map of 691 foci resulting from general semantic contrasts (see Methods for details). Formatting and thresholding as in Figure 3.

Specific Contrasts

The specific contrasts were further categorized as to the putative type of semantic knowledge examined by each. Many of these types (e.g., auditory, gustatory, olfactory, tactile, visual motion, characteristic object location, emotion, causation, and self knowledge) were examined in only a few studies and thus had too few activation foci to examine separately by meta-analysis. There were 10 studies that examined contrasts between living things (usually animals) and manipulable artifacts (usually tools) (Mummery et al. 1996, 1998; Cappa et al. 1998; Perani et al. 1999; Grossman et al. 2002a; Laine et al. 2002; Kounios et al. 2003; Thioux et al. 2005; Wheatley et al. 2005; Goldberg et al. 2006), providing 41 “living” and 29 “artifact” foci. “Living” foci showed no significant overlap in the ALE analysis. As shown in Figure 5, “artifact” foci showed significant overlap at the left lateral temporal–occipital junction, where posterior MTG and ITG meet anterior occipital cortex (roughly BA 37), and in the ventral left SMG (BA 40) near the junction with the superior temporal gyrus (STG).

ALE map of 29 foci resulting from contrasts targeting knowledge of manipulable artifacts (top) and ALE map of 40 foci from contrasts targeting knowledge of actions (bottom). Formatting and thresholding as in previous figures.
Figure 5.

ALE map of 29 foci resulting from contrasts targeting knowledge of manipulable artifacts (top) and ALE map of 40 foci from contrasts targeting knowledge of actions (bottom). Formatting and thresholding as in previous figures.

There were 10 studies that examined action knowledge relative to other types (Martin et al. 1995; Noppeney and Price 2003; Tyler, Stamatakis, et al. 2003; Davis et al. 2004; Boronat et al. 2005; Noppeney et al. 2005; Baumgaertner et al. 2007; Eschen et al. 2007; Ruschmeyer et al. 2007; Tomasino et al. 2007), providing 40 “action” foci. Significant overlap for these foci occurred in the ventral left SMG and posterior left MTG (BA 37). As shown in Figure 5, the SMG focus overlaps the SMG region observed in the “artifact” studies. The posterior MTG cluster associated with action knowledge was slightly dorsal and lateral to the temporal–occipital cluster of “artifact” foci.

There were 17 studies that examined the distinction between perceptually encoded knowledge (i.e., knowledge of concrete objects derived from sensory-motor experience) and verbally encoded knowledge (i.e., knowledge acquired through language) (Paivio 1986). The majority of these studies used contrasts between concrete and abstract concepts (Jessen et al. 2000; Wise et al. 2000; Grossman et al. 2002b; Fiebach and Friederici 2003; Giesbrecht et al. 2004; Noppeney and Price 2004; Whatmough et al. 2004; Binder, Medler, et al. 2005; Binder, Westbury, et al. 2005;,Sabsevitz et al. 2005; Wallentin, Østergaard, et al. 2005; Bedny and Thompson-Schill 2006; Fliessbach et al. 2006), whereas a few others examined this distinction using tasks that required explicit knowledge of perceptual versus verbal facts (Fletcher et al. 1995; Noppeney and Price 2003; Lee and Dapretto 2006; Ebisch et al. 2007). Significant overlap for the 113 “perceptual” foci occurred in the AG bilaterally, left mid-fusiform gyrus, left DMPFC, and left posterior cingulate (Fig. 6). Significant overlap for the 34 “verbal” foci occurred in the left IFG (mainly pars orbitalis) and left anterior superior temporal sulcus (STS).

ALE maps derived from contrasts comparing perceptual (i.e., pertaining to sensory attributes of concrete objects) with verbal (i.e., abstract or encyclopedic) knowledge. The 113 foci representing perceptual knowledge are shown in warm colors, and the 34 foci representing verbal knowledge are shown in cool colors. Formatting and thresholding as in previous figures.
Figure 6.

ALE maps derived from contrasts comparing perceptual (i.e., pertaining to sensory attributes of concrete objects) with verbal (i.e., abstract or encyclopedic) knowledge. The 113 foci representing perceptual knowledge are shown in warm colors, and the 34 foci representing verbal knowledge are shown in cool colors. Formatting and thresholding as in previous figures.

Discussion

PET and fMRI activation studies are based, directly or indirectly, on differences between 2 or more brain activity states. The “activation maps” produced by these methods represent relative changes in brain activity, not absolute activity levels. Interpretation of such activations, therefore, cannot be based solely on the processing demands of one of the task states, but rather requires a joint analysis of the processing demands elicited by each of the task states and the degree to which they differ. The goal of the present meta-analysis was to clarify the brain regions specifically involved in semantic processing, a topic that has been the source of much debate (for a sample of conflicting views, see Wernicke 1874; Head 1926; Petersen et al. 1988; Démonet et al. 1993; Thompson-Schill et al. 1997; Tranel et al. 1997; Hillis et al. 2001; Martin and Caramazza 2003; Patterson et al. 2007). In contrast to previous meta-analyses on this topic, we used explicit criteria to define activation representing semantic processing; these criteria referred to differences between the stimuli and tasks used to generate each activation map. To be considered for inclusion, a contrast had to involve a difference in either the degree to which stored knowledge was accessed (“general” contrast) or the specific type of knowledge accessed (“specific” contrast). These differences in stored knowledge access could be elicited through manipulation of stimulus characteristics (e.g., words vs. pseudowords, meaningful vs. meaningless sentences, famous vs. unfamiliar names, and animals vs. tools), through manipulation of the subject's attention via task instructions (e.g., semantic vs. phonological decisions, color vs. action decisions), or both.

Another central feature of the present study that distinguishes it from previous reviews was the application of strict exclusion criteria. To minimize contamination of the results by nonsemantic processes, studies were excluded if the semantic condition of interest also made greater demands on low-level sensory, orthographic, phonological, syntactic, working memory, attentional, response selection, or motor processes. (Note that studies were excluded only when the semantic condition of interest made greater demands on these processes, not when the comparison condition made greater demands.) The 2 most common reasons for exclusion were inadequate controls for phonological processing due to use of unpronounceable or nonlinguistic stimuli in the comparison condition, and inadequate controls for general task performance processes. The latter type of exclusion was the most common and warrants further discussion, because in our view many prior studies have not clearly distinguished knowledge access processes from more general cognitive processes that are not specific to semantic tasks. The critical point we wish to make is that all consciously executed, goal-directed tasks require at minimum a set of domain-general processes that include maintenance of attention, direction of attention to relevant information (external or internal), maintenance of this relevant information in a short-term memory store, maintenance of the task goal and task procedures in working memory, decision, response selection, and error monitoring. These processes are necessary for all goal-directed cognitive tasks, including semantic tasks; however, our aim here was to identify brain regions engaged specifically in semantic processes. Thus, it was critical to exclude activation contrasts in which the semantic condition of interest engaged these processes to a greater degree than the comparison condition, including all contrasts in which the semantic task was more difficult than the comparison task. In addition to the examples given in the introduction, another illustrative case are the many studies involving either semantic priming or repetition suppression, in which unprimed or new words are compared with primed or repeated words (e.g., Mummery et al. 1999; Wagner et al. 2000; Yasuno et al. 2000; Rossell et al. 2001; Copland et al. 2003; Rossell et al. 2003; Matsumoto et al. 2005; Wible et al. 2006). One underlying hypothesis of these studies is that unprimed or new words require more semantic processing than primed or repeated words that have already been processed, and behavioral data uniformly support this hypothesis by showing longer response times for the unprimed/new items. Though it is likely that unprimed/new items elicit more extensive semantic processing, it is also an inescapable fact, in our view, that they also require greater attentional and executive resources. Exclusion of these studies from the present meta-analysis was therefore necessary to isolate the semantic processes of interest, even though the activation maps from these contrasts probably do reflect, at least in part, semantic processes.

The Semantic System of the Human Brain

The meta-analysis links the following 7 brain regions with semantic processes: 1) posterior inferior parietal lobe (AG and portions of SMG), 2) lateral temporal cortex (MTG and portions of ITG), 3) ventral temporal cortex (mid-fusiform and adjacent parahippocampal gyrus), 4) DMPFC, 5) IFG, 6) ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC), and 7) posterior cingulate gyrus. One common attribute of these regions is their likely role in high-level integrative processes. All are known to receive extensively processed, multimodal and supramodal input. Recent studies show that even cortical regions formerly considered “unimodal” receive multisensory inputs (Schroeder and Foxe 2004; Cappe and Barone 2005), blurring the traditional distinction between unimodal and heteromodal cortex (Mesulam 1985). A useful qualitative distinction can still be drawn, however, between “modal” cortex, where processing reflects a dominant sensory or motor modality, and “amodal” cortex, where input from multiple modalities is more nearly balanced and highly convergent. For continuity with previous work, we refer to these latter regions as heteromodal, though alternative terms such as supramodal or amodal are perhaps equally valid. The human semantic system thus corresponds in large measure to the network of parietal, temporal, and prefrontal heteromodal association areas, which are greatly expanded in the human relative to the nonhuman primate brain (von Bonin 1962; Geschwind 1965; Brodmann 1994/1909). Evidence supports the subdivision of this network into posterior (temporal/parietal) and frontal components corresponding to storage and retrieval aspects of semantic processing (see discussion below). A second general feature of the semantic system is that it is lateralized to the left hemisphere, though with some bilateral representation (particularly in the AG and posterior cingulate gyrus). The following discussion reviews each of the nodes in this network in greater detail, examining their anatomical characteristics and likely functional roles based on imaging and neuropsychological data.

Angular Gyrus

The most dense concentration of activation foci was in the posterior aspect of the left inferior parietal lobule, a region known historically as the angular gyrus or “pli courbe” (French: “curved gyrus”) (Déjerine 1895). The AG consists of cortex surrounding the parietal extension of the STS; it is formed essentially by the continuation of the superior and middle temporal gyri into the inferior parietal lobe. Its medial boundary is the intraparietal sulcus, which separates it from the superior parietal lobule. The anterior boundary with the SMG is defined by the first intermediate sulcus of Jensen, though this landmark is not always present. Its posterior boundary with the occipital lobe is not well defined. The AG corresponds approximately to BA 39 and in recent cytoarchitectonic studies to PGa and PGp (Caspers et al. 2006). This region is practically nonexistent in lower primates (Brodmann 1994/1909) and is greatly expanded in the human brain relative to its probable homolog in the macaque monkey, area PG/7a (von Bonin and Bailey 1947; Hyvarinen 1982). It is anatomically connected almost entirely with other association regions and receives little or no direct input from primary sensory areas (Mesulam et al. 1977; Hyvarinen 1982; Seltzer and Pandya 1984; Cavada and Goldman-Rakic 1989a, 1989b; Andersen et al. 1990).

Though we use the term angular gyrus, a variety of other labels for activations in this region were encountered in the studies reviewed. Despite its location in the parietal lobe, many refer to it erroneously as the middle temporal gyrus. Others use the terms temporoparietal junction or temporal–parietal–occipital cortex. These concatenated terms strike us as unnecessarily imprecise in this context and should probably be reserved for describing large activations that straddle the boundaries between lobes or extend beyond the parietal lobe. AG activations were also not infrequently mislabeled with BA numbers 40 and 19. (Some of this confusion may be a historical accident stemming from Brodmann's famous illustration, which shows area 39 shrunken to a fraction of its true size relative to surrounding structures [Brodmann 1994/1909]. Other cytoarchitectonic studies have portrayed this region as much more extensive [von Economo and Koskinas 1925; Sarkissov et al. 1955]. Brodmann's intent seems to have been to show both lateral and dorsal brain regions in a single lateral view. This required that the inferior parietal lobule on the lateral surface be reduced in size to accommodate areas on the superior parietal lobule, which are normally not well seen from a lateral view.)

On the other hand, some of the activation foci in this large cluster probably lie outside the AG. Several are just posterior, in what is likely BA 19. Given this evidence from functional imaging, it is possible that at least some cortex in the anterior occipital lobe classically identified as BA 19 may serve a semantic rather than a modal visual associative function. Alternatively, BA 39 may extend farther posteriorly than is typically portrayed. Several other foci in this large cluster were in the SMG (BA 40) just anterior to the AG.

Lesions of the left AG produce a variety of cognitive deficits, including alexia and agraphia (Déjerine 1892; Benson 1979; Cipolotti et al. 1991), anomia (Benson 1979), transcortical sensory aphasia (Damasio 1981; Kertesz et al. 1982; Rapcsak and Rubens 1994), sentence comprehension impairment (Dronkers et al. 2004), acalculia (Gerstmann 1940; Benton 1961; Cipolotti et al. 1991; Dehaene and Cohen 1997), visual-spatial and body schema disorders (Gerstmann 1940; Critchley 1953), ideomotor apraxia (Haaland et al. 2000; Buxbaum et al. 2005; Jax et al. 2006), and dementia (Benson et al. 1982). Perhaps the main conclusion to be drawn from this evidence is that the AG likely plays a role in complex information integration and knowledge retrieval. Given its anatomical location adjoining visual, spatial, auditory, and somatosensory association areas, the AG may be the single best candidate for a high-level, supramodal integration area in the human brain (Geschwind 1965). Several functional imaging studies have shown that the AG is activated in response to semantically anomalous words embedded in sentences, suggesting that it plays a role in integrating individual concepts into a larger whole (Ni et al. 2000; Friederici et al. 2003; Newman et al. 2003). One recent fMRI study found that during auditory sentence comprehension, the AG, alone among the regions activated, showed a late activation relative to baseline that began at the end of the sentence and occurred only when the constituent words could be integrated into a coherent meaning (Humphries et al. 2007). Three studies comparing processing of connected discourse to processing of unrelated sentences or phrases have also shown activation of the AG (Fletcher et al. 1995; Homae et al. 2003; Xu et al. 2005) Considering these various lines of evidence, we propose that the AG occupies a position at the top of a processing hierarchy underlying concept retrieval and conceptual integration. Though it is involved in all aspects of semantic processing, it may play a particular role in behaviors requiring fluent conceptual combination, such as sentence comprehension, discourse, problem solving, and planning.

Lateral and Ventral Temporal Cortex

The meta-analysis identified several regions in the lateral and ventral left temporal lobe, including most of the MTG and portions of the ITG, fusiform gyrus, and parahippocampus. MTG, ITG, and ventral temporal lobe have often been considered modal visual association cortex by analogy with lateral and ventral temporal cortex in the macaque monkey (von Bonin and Bailey 1947; Mesulam 1985); however, the present analysis argues against such an interpretation in the human brain. In fact, many functional imaging studies have demonstrated activation of these regions by auditory stimuli, particularly during language tasks (e.g., Démonet et al. 1992; Binder et al. 1997; Wise et al. 2000; Noppeney et al. 2003; Rissman et al. 2003; von Kriegstein et al. 2003; Xiao et al. 2005; Humphries et al. 2006; Orfanidou et al. 2006; Spitsyna et al. 2006; Baumgaertner et al. 2007). Thus, these regions in the human brain are likely heteromodal cortex involved in supramodal integration and concept retrieval. As in the inferior parietal lobe, the relative expansion of this high-level integrative cortex in the temporal lobe has resulted in modal visual cortex being “pushed” posteriorly and reduced in relative surface area (Orban et al. 2004).

Focal damage to the MTG, though somewhat rare, is strongly associated with language comprehension and semantic deficits (e.g., Hart and Gordon 1990; Hillis and Caramazza 1991; Kertesz et al. 1993; Chertkow et al. 1997; Dronkers et al. 2004). The anterior ventral temporal lobe, including anterior MTG, ITG, and fusiform gyrus, is frequently damaged (usually bilaterally) in herpes simplex encephalitis, often resulting in profound semantic deficits (Warrington and Shallice 1984; Kapur, Barker, et al. 1994;,Gitelman et al. 2001; Lambon Ralph et al. 2007; Noppeney et al. 2007). Semantic dementia, the temporal lobe variant of frontotemporal dementia, is characterized by progressive degeneration of the anterior ventrolateral temporal lobes and gradual loss of semantic knowledge (Warrington 1975; Snowden et al. 1989; Hodges et al. 1992, 1995; Mummery et al. 2000; Jefferies and Lambon Ralph 2006; Lambon Ralph et al. 2007; Noppeney et al. 2007). Large lesions of the ventral left temporal lobe have been associated with transcortical sensory aphasia (Damasio 1981; Kertesz et al. 1982; Alexander et al. 1989; Berthier 1999). A striking aspect of many of these temporal lobe injuries is a dissociation in performance across object categories. Patients with anterior temporal damage, for example, occasionally show greater impairment in processing concepts related to living things compared with artifacts (Warrington and Shallice 1984; Warrington and McCarthy 1987; Forde and Humphreys 1999; Gainotti 2000; Lambon Ralph et al. 2007), and the opposite pattern has been reported in patients with posterior temporal and parietal lesions (Warrington and McCarthy 1987, 1994; Hillis and Caramazza 1991; Gainotti 2000). These category-related deficits suggest that the temporal lobe may be a principal site for storage of perceptual information about objects and their attributes. A large number of functional imaging studies provide support for this hypothesis by showing selective activation of the posterior lateral temporal lobe by tool and action concepts (Martin et al. 1995, 1996; Cappa et al. 1998; Chao et al. 1999, 2002; Moore and Price 1999a; Perani et al. 1999; Grossman et al. 2002a; Kable et al. 2002; Phillips et al. 2002; Noppeney et al. 2003; Tyler, Stamatakis, et al. 2003; Davis et al. 2004; Kable et al. 2005; Noppeney et al. 2005; Wallentin, Lund et al. 2005).

Semantic foci in the fusiform and parahippocampal gyri were concentrated in a relatively focal region near the mid-point of these gyri, centered at y = −35 in the Talairach–Tournoux system. The specific role of this region is unknown. It may correspond to the “basal temporal language area” described in electrocortical stimulation mapping studies (Lüders et al. 1991). It is anterior to activation sites observed in functional imaging studies comparing different categories of object pictures (e.g., Perani et al. 1995; Martin et al. 1996; Kanwisher et al. 1997; Epstein and Kanwisher 1998; Chao et al. 1999; Ishai et al. 1999; Moore and Price 1999a; Gorno-Tempini et al. 2000; Okada et al. 2000; Haxby et al. 2001; Chao et al. 2002; Whatmough et al. 2002; Tyler, Bright, et al. 2003; Gerlach et al. 2004; Gerlach 2007). These more posterior activations (typically y < −50) are rarely observed in studies using words, suggesting that they arise from systematic differences between object categories in their constituent visual attributes, which are in turn processed by somewhat different visual perceptual mechanisms (Humphreys and Forde 2001; Hasson et al. 2002). Given its close proximity to these object perception areas, however, several authors have proposed that the mid-fusiform gyrus plays a particular role in retrieving knowledge about the visual attributes of concrete objects (D'Esposito et al. 1997; Chao and Martin 1999; Thompson-Schill, Aguirre, et al. 1999; Wise et al. 2000; Kan et al. 2003; Vandenbulcke et al. 2006; Simmons et al. 2007). This region is also near the hippocampus and massive cortical afferent pathways to the hippocampal formation via parahippocampal and entorhinal cortex (Van Hoesen 1982; Insausti et al. 1987; Suzuki and Amaral 1994). It is thus possible that the parahippocampal component of this cluster acts an interface between lateral semantic memory and medial episodic memory encoding networks (Levy et al. 2004).

The present analysis provides little evidence for involvement of the STG in semantic processing. The STG has long been considered to play a central role in language comprehension (e.g., Wernicke 1874; Geschwind 1971; Bogen and Bogen 1976; Hillis et al. 2001), but anatomical and functional data suggest that it contains mainly modal auditory cortex (von Economo and Koskinas 1925; Galaburda and Sanides 1980; Baylis et al. 1987; Kaas and Hackett 2000; Poremba et al. 2003). Its role in language relates primarily to speech perception and phonological processing rather than to retrieval of word meaning (Henschen 1918–1919; Binder et al. 2000; Wise et al. 2001; Binder 2002; Hickok et al. 2003; Scott and Johnsrude 2003; Indefrey and Levelt 2004; Liebenthal et al. 2005; Buchsbaum and D'Esposito 2008; Graves et al. 2008). Several studies, however, suggest that portions of the left superior temporal sulcus, which includes ventral STG, play a role in processing abstract concepts (see below).

Left DMPFC

We draw particular attention to this region, which has been largely overlooked in reviews on semantic processing despite its consistent activation. It forms a distinctive, diagonally oriented band extending from the posterior–medial aspect of the MFG, across the superior frontal sulcus and dorsal SFG, and onto the medial surface of the SFG. It corresponds roughly to BA 8, extending into BA 9 medially. We use the term “dorsomedial” to distinguish this region from “dorsolateral prefrontal cortex” located lateral and ventral to it in the lateral MFG and inferior frontal sulcus.

Lesions of the left dorsal and medial frontal lobe cause transcortical motor aphasia, a syndrome characterized by sparse speech output but otherwise normal phonological abilities (Luria and Tsvetkova 1968; Freedman et al. 1984; Alexander and Benson 1993). There is typically a striking disparity between cued and uncued speech production in this syndrome. Patients can repeat words and name objects relatively normally, but are unable to generate lists of words within a category or invent nonformulaic responses in conversation. In other words, patients perform well when a simple response is fully specified by the stimulus (a word to be repeated or object to be named) but poorly when a large set of responses is possible (Robinson et al. 1998). This pattern suggests a deficit specifically affecting self-guided, goal-directed retrieval of semantic information. The location of the DMPFC, adjacent to motivation and sustained attention networks in the anterior cingulate gyrus and just anterior to premotor cortex, makes this region a likely candidate for this semantic retrieval role.

The left DMPFC has not been delineated in previous discussions of the prefrontal cortex and semantic retrieval processes. Analysis of medial frontal lesions in transcortical motor aphasia has usually centered on the supplementary motor area (SMA), a region of medial premotor cortex (BA 6) posterior to the DMPFC, perhaps because of the attention drawn to this region in earlier stimulation mapping studies (Penfield and Roberts 1959). Some authors, citing the involvement of SMA and anterior cingulate cortex in motor planning, attention, and motivation processes, dismissed the deficits in patients with left medial frontal lesions as nonlinguistic in nature (Damasio 1981). Others have recognized the linguistic nature of the retrieval deficit while attributing this to SMA damage (Masdeu et al. 1978; Freedman et al. 1984; Goldberg 1985). The DMPFC and SMA have a common arterial supply (the callosomarginal branch of the anterior cerebral artery) and for this reason are usually damaged together in ischemic lesions. Although we agree that focal SMA damage is unlikely to produce a linguistic deficit, we propose that the specific linguistic deficit affecting fluent semantic retrieval in many of these patients is due to DMPFC damage anterior to the SMA.

Left IFG

The left IFG was implicated in several early imaging studies of semantic processing (Petersen et al. 1988; Frith et al. 1991; Kapur, Rose, et al. 1994), and much subsequent discussion has focused on this region (e.g., Démonet et al. 1993; Buckner et al. 1995; Fiez 1997; Thompson-Schill et al. 1997; Gabrieli et al. 1998; Poldrack et al. 1999; Thompson-Schill et al. 1999; Wagner et al. 2000; Roskies et al. 2001; Wagner et al. 2001; Bookheimer 2002; Chee et al. 2002; Gold and Buckner 2002; Devlin et al. 2003; Nyberg et al. 2003; Simmons et al. 2005; Goldberg et al. 2007). Consistent with prior reviews (Fiez 1997; Bookheimer 2002), the meta-analysis shows clear involvement of the anterior–ventral left IFG in semantic processing. This region corresponds to the “pars orbitalis” (BA 47). More posterior and dorsal parts of the IFG were also activated, though less consistently.

Imaging studies have also frequently implicated the left IFG in phonological, working memory, and syntactic processes (e.g., Démonet et al. 1992; Zatorre et al. 1992; Paulesu et al. 1993; Buckner et al. 1995; Fiez 1997; Smith et al. 1998; Fiez et al. 1999; Poldrack et al. 1999; Burton et al. 2000; Embick et al. 2000; Poldrack et al. 2001; Gold and Buckner 2002; Friederici et al. 2003; Nyberg et al. 2003; Davis et al. 2004; Indefrey and Levelt 2004; Fiebach et al. 2005; Owen et al. 2005; Tan et al. 2005; Grodzinsky and Friederici 2006). Many studies have also shown increased BOLD responses in the IFG as task difficulty increases, possibly due to increased working memory or phonological processing demands (e.g., Braver et al. 1997, 2001; Jonides et al. 1997; Honey et al. 2000; Adler et al. 2001; Ullsperger and von Cramon 2001; Gould et al. 2003; Binder, Medler, et al. 2005; Mitchell 2005; Sabsevitz et al. 2005; Desai et al. 2006; Lehmann et al. 2006; Tregallas et al. 2006). Though we attempted to remove contrasts in which semantic processing was confounded with phonological processing or overall difficulty, these screening efforts were likely imperfect due to the absence of appropriate behavioral data in many published studies. It is thus possible that some of the IFG activation foci, particularly those outside the pars orbitalis, are the result of residual phonological or working memory confounds.

As is well known, IFG lesions typically impair phonological, articulatory planning, and syntactic rather than semantic processes (Broca 1861; Mohr 1976; Caramazza et al. 1981; Alexander and Benson 1993), though a few cases of transcortical sensory aphasia have been reported (Otsuki et al. 1998; Maeshima et al. 1999, 2004; Sethi et al. 2007). Strokes affect the posterior aspect of the IFG more commonly than the anterior region; isolated lesions of the pars orbitalis are practically unknown. Devlin et al. (2003) applied transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to the anterior IFG in 8 healthy participants during performance of semantic decision and perceptual (size) decision tasks. TMS slowed participants’ reaction time on the semantic but not on the control task, supporting a role for this region in semantic processing. Accuracy on the semantic task, however, was not affected by TMS. It may be that the anterior–ventral IFG contributes to semantic processing in the healthy brain but is not absolutely necessary for task completion (Price et al. 1999). Damage to this region thus impairs processing “efficiency,” resulting in slowing of responses without actual errors.

Left VMPFC

This group of foci occupy cortex in the cingulate gyrus and medial SFG anterior to the genu of the corpus callosum, the subgenual cingulate gyrus, gyrus rectus, and medial orbital frontal cortex. The involved region of anterior cingulate cortex is anterior and ventral to the more dorsal region of anterior cingulate cortex implicated in many studies of working memory, response conflict, error detection, and executive control functions (e.g., Carter et al. 1999; Duncan and Owen 2000; Barch et al. 2001; van Veen and Carter 2002; Owen et al. 2005). We use the term “rostral cingulate gyrus” to emphasize this distinction. The VMPFC corresponds to portions of BA 10, 11, 24, 25, and 32. This region has been linked with motivation, emotion, and reward processing and probably plays a central role in processing the affective significance of concepts (Damasio 1994; Drevets et al. 1997; Mayberg et al. 1999; Bechara et al. 2000; Phillips et al. 2003). It has also been activated in many general semantic contrasts, however, possibly due to incidental processing of the emotional attributes of words (Kuchinke et al. 2005).

Posterior Cingulate Gyrus

This region, which corresponds to BA 23, BA 31, and the retrosplenial region (BA 26, 29, and 30), was one of the most consistently activated. Activation peaks occurred in both hemispheres but more often on the left. A few foci in this cluster were located in the ventral aspect of the precuneus just dorsal to the posterior cingulate, in the region of the subparietal sulcus separating these gyri, or in the ventral parieto-occipital sulcus, which separates the posterior cingulate gyrus from the occipital lobe.

This general region has been linked with episodic and visuospatial memory functions (Valenstein et al. 1987; Rudge and Warrington 1991; Gainotti et al. 1998; Aggleton and Pearce 2001; Vincent et al. 2006; Epstein et al. 2007), emotion processing (Maddock 1999), spatial attention (Mesulam 1990; Small et al. 2003), visual imagery (Hassabis et al. 2007; Johnson et al. 2007; Burgess 2008), and other processes (Vogt et al. 2006). Of these, the association with episodic memory may be most likely. Posterior cingulate and adjacent retrosplenial cortex have strong reciprocal connections with the hippocampal complex via the cingulum bundle (Morris et al. 1999; Kobayashi and Amaral 2003, 2007). A number of patients with focal lesions to this region have presented with amnestic syndromes (Valenstein et al. 1987; Heilman et al. 1990; Rudge and Warrington 1991; Takayama et al. 1991; Katai et al. 1992; Gainotti et al. 1998; McDonald et al. 2001). Retrosplenial and surrounding posterior cingulate cortex are affected early in the course of Alzheimer disease, which typically presents as an episodic memory encoding deficit (Desgranges et al. 2002; Nestor et al. 2003).

If posterior cingulate cortex is involved primarily in encoding episodic memories, why is it consistently activated in contrasts that emphasize semantic processing? The likely answer has to do with the nature of episodic memory, the presumed evolutionary purpose of which is to form a record of past experience for use in guiding future behavior. Not all experiences are equally useful in this regard; thus, the brain has evolved a strategy of preferentially recording highly meaningful experiences, that is, experiences that evoke associations and concepts. Familiar examples of this phenomenon include the enhanced learning of words encoded during semantic relative to perceptual tasks, imageable relative to abstract words, and emotional relative to neutral words (Paivio 1968; Craik 1972 #762; Bock 1986). In each case, the enhanced retrieval of conceptual information (semantic retrieval) leads to enhanced episodic encoding. Several related theories of this phenomenon have been proposed (Cohen and Eichenbaum 1993; McClelland et al. 1995; O'Reilly and Rudy 2001), all of which postulate that episodic memory encoding involves the formation of large-scale representations through interactions between neocortex and the hippocampal system. The role of the neocortex is to compute ongoing perceptual, semantic, affective, and motor representations during the episode, while the hippocampal system binds these spatiotemporal cortical events into a unique event configuration. The important point is that the amount of episodic encoding that occurs is highly correlated with the degree of semantic processing evoked by the episode. We propose that the posterior cingulate gyrus, by virtue of its strong connections with the hippocampus, acts as an interface between the semantic retrieval and episodic encoding systems, similar to the role postulated above for the parahippocampal gyrus.

Homologues of the Human Semantic System in the Macaque Monkey Brain

The posterior inferior parietal lobe of the macaque monkey, variously designated 7a (Vogt and Vogt 1919) or PG (von Bonin and Bailey 1947), and more recently subdivided into 2 subregions, PG and Opt (Pandya and Seltzer 1982; Gregoriou et al. 2006), is a likely homologue of the human AG with similar heteromodal functional characteristics (Hyvarinen 1982). Its principal connections are with visual and “polysensory” regions in the upper bank and fundus of the STS (areas TPO, STP, MST, and IPa), the parahippocampal gyrus (areas TF and TH), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (mainly area 46), rostrolateral orbitofrontal cortex (area 11), and posterior cingulate gyrus (Jones and Powell 1970; Mesulam et al. 1977; Leichnitz 1980; Petrides and Pandya 1984; Seltzer and Pandya 1984, 1994; Selemon and Goldman-Rakic 1988; Cavada and Goldman-Rakic 1989a, 1989b; Andersen et al. 1990). Notably, the same STS, parahippocampal, prefrontal, and posterior cingulate regions with which PG/7a is connected are themselves all strongly interconnected (Jones and Powell 1970; Seltzer and Pandya 1976, 1978, 1989, 1994; Baleydier and Mauguiere 1980; Vogt and Pandya 1987; Selemon and Goldman-Rakic 1988; Morris et al. 1999; Blatt et al. 2003; Kobayashi and Amaral 2003, 2007; Padberg et al. 2003; Parvizi et al. 2006). These 6 regions thus form a distinct, large-scale cortical network that is strikingly similar in location and function to the human semantic system (Fig. 7). The other chief component of the human system, VMPFC, is roughly homologous to the medial orbitofrontal (BA 10, 14, 25, 32) region of the macaque. Although this region has no connection with PG/7a, it is strongly connected to middle and anterior STS, posterior cingulate and retrosplenial cortex, parahippocampus, and hippocampus (Seltzer and Pandya 1989; Barbas 1993; Cavada 2000; Blatt et al. 2003; Kobayashi and Amaral 2003; Saleem et al. 2007). Thus, the macaque brain contains a well-defined network of polysensory, heteromodal, and paralimbic areas that are several processing stages removed from primary sensory and motor regions and likely to be involved in computation of complex, nonperceptual information. We propose that this network is a nonhuman primate homologue of the human semantic system, responsible for storage of abstract knowledge about conspecifics, food sources, objects, actions, and emotions. Anatomical differences between the human and macaque systems are consistent with the known expansion of prefrontal, parietal, and temporal heteromodal cortex in the human brain, which has enabled in humans further abstraction of knowledge from perceptual events, ultimately culminating in the development of formal symbol systems to represent and communicate this knowledge.

Summary diagrams comparing (A) the large-scale semantic network of the human brain and (B) a probable homologous network in the macaque monkey brain, comprised of posterior inferior parietal cortex (PG/7a), STS, parahippocampal cortex (TF, TH), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate and retrosplenial cortex, lateral orbital frontal cortex, and VMPFC. Green lines indicate the principal cortical connections of these regions in the monkey, based on studies using anterograde and retrograde tracer techniques (Jones and Powell 1970; Seltzer and Pandya 1976, 1978, 1984, 1989, 1994; Mesulam et al. 1977; Baleydier and Mauguiere 1980; Leichnitz 1980; Petrides and Pandya 1984; Vogt and Pandya 1987; Selemon and Goldman-Rakic 1988; Cavada and Goldman-Rakic 1989a, 1989b; Andersen et al. 1990; Barbas 1993; Morris et al. 1999; Cavada 2000; Blatt et al. 2003; Kobayashi and Amaral 2003, 2007; Padberg et al. 2003; Parvizi et al. 2006; Saleem et al. 2007). All connections indicated are monosynaptic and reciprocal.
Figure 7.

Summary diagrams comparing (A) the large-scale semantic network of the human brain and (B) a probable homologous network in the macaque monkey brain, comprised of posterior inferior parietal cortex (PG/7a), STS, parahippocampal cortex (TF, TH), dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate and retrosplenial cortex, lateral orbital frontal cortex, and VMPFC. Green lines indicate the principal cortical connections of these regions in the monkey, based on studies using anterograde and retrograde tracer techniques (Jones and Powell 1970; Seltzer and Pandya 1976, 1978, 1984, 1989, 1994; Mesulam et al. 1977; Baleydier and Mauguiere 1980; Leichnitz 1980; Petrides and Pandya 1984; Vogt and Pandya 1987; Selemon and Goldman-Rakic 1988; Cavada and Goldman-Rakic 1989a, 1989b; Andersen et al. 1990; Barbas 1993; Morris et al. 1999; Cavada 2000; Blatt et al. 2003; Kobayashi and Amaral 2003, 2007; Padberg et al. 2003; Parvizi et al. 2006; Saleem et al. 2007). All connections indicated are monosynaptic and reciprocal.

The macaque parietal/frontal/STS network illustrated in Figure 7 has often been interpreted as playing a central role in visuospatial processing and spatial allocation of attention (Mesulam 1981; Hyvarinen 1982; Seltzer and Pandya 1984; Selemon and Goldman-Rakic 1988). This view is supported by a large number of studies showing cells in the posterior inferior parietal lobe of the macaque that respond to oculomotor, limb movement, and spatial attention tasks (Mountcastle et al. 1975; Hyvarinen 1982; Andersen et al. 1997). This model is clearly at odds, however, with our proposal that these regions are involved in long-term storage and retrieval of object and action knowledge. In our view, the characterization of this network as visuospatial/attentional does not account for the prominent connections of these frontoparietal areas with polysensory and paralimbic areas. We believe these models can be reconciled by a consideration of known subdivisions of the macaque posterior parietal lobe. Research over the past 20 years has clarified the connectivity and functional properties of several areas immediately anteromedial to PG/7a in the macaque intraparietal sulcus (LIP, VIP, and MIP), which appear to play a greater role in visuospatial and attention processes than PG/7a (Andersen et al. 1990, 1997; Rushworth et al. 1997; Chafee and Goldman-Rakic 1998; Duhamel et al. 1998). Unlike PG/7a, these IPS regions have little or no connectivity with the temporal lobe or paralimbic regions (Seltzer and Pandya 1984; Cavada and Goldman-Rakic 1989a; Suzuki and Amaral 1994). They are connected strongly to the frontal eye fields and premotor cortex, whereas PG/7a is connected to more anterior and dorsal prefrontal regions (area 46) (Cavada and Goldman-Rakic 1989b; Andersen et al. 1990). Numerous functional imaging studies have also clearly linked the IPS and frontal eye fields in humans with visuospatial and attention functions (Corbetta et al. 1998; Grefkes and Fink 2005; Grosbras et al. 2005). Thus, we propose that the posterior parietal spatial attention system in both the human and macaque is confined mainly to cortex in the IPS and superior parietal lobule, and that there is a distinct functional and anatomical boundary between this IPS system and adjacent inferior parietal cortex involved in semantic knowledge representation. This boundary line appears to correspond in both species to the superior margin of the lateral (posterior) bank of the IPS (see Fig. 2).

Evidence for Distinct Semantic Subsystems

In addition to brain networks supporting semantic processing in general, particular regions may be relatively specialized for processing specific object categories, attributes, or types of knowledge. Prior reviews on this topic have included studies that used object pictures as stimuli, whereas the present meta-analysis was confined to studies using words. The number of such studies that examined specific types of semantic knowledge was relatively small, and activation peaks from these studies showed little overlap. The clearest pattern emerged from the 10 studies examining action knowledge (Fig. 5). Two distinct activation clusters were observed in left SMG and posterior MTG. Lesions in these areas have been associated with impairments of action knowledge and ideomotor apraxia in many neuropsychological studies (Tranel et al. 1997, 2003; Haaland et al. 2000; Buxbaum et al. 2005; Jax et al. 2006). The SMG focus lies just posterior to somatosensory association cortex; thus, it seems likely that this region stores abstract somatosensory (e.g., proprioceptive) knowledge acquired during learning and performance of complex motor sequences. The likely homologue of this region in the macaque monkey is area PF in the anterior inferior parietal lobe, a region known to contain mirror neurons responsive to both action observation and performance (Rizzolatti and Craighero 2004). Activation of this region in humans by words, which merely refer conceptually to actions, lends support to the idea that the information stored there is semantic in nature, coding complex actions performed on objects for a specific purpose (Rothi et al. 1991; Buxbaum 2001; Buxbaum et al. 2005, 2006; Fogassi et al. 2005). The posterior MTG focus is just anterior to visual motion processing areas in the MT complex (Tootell et al. 1995), suggesting that this region stores knowledge about the visual attributes of actions. As previously suggested (Martin et al. 2000), this specialization of posterior MTG for processing action knowledge may explain the frequently observed preferential activation of this region by pictures of tools (Martin et al. 1996; Mummery et al. 1996; Cappa et al. 1998; Chao et al. 1999, 2002; Moore and Price 1999a; Perani et al. 1999; Devlin, Moore, et al. 2002; Phillips et al. 2002; Damasio et al. 2004). Indeed, the studies examining knowledge of manipulable artifacts (relative to living things) produced areas of overlap at very similar sites in the SMG and near the junction of posterior MTG, ITG, and lateral occipital lobe. In contrast, the studies examining knowledge of living things (usually animals) relative to other categories produced no significant areas of overlap, consistent with several prior reviews (Devlin, Russell, et al. 2002;,Gerlach 2007).

Given the presence of mirror neurons in premotor cortex (Rizzolatti and Craighero 2004) and prior imaging evidence that the inferior frontal region responds to pictures of manipulable objects (Martin et al. 1996; Binkofski et al. 1999; Perani et al. 1999; Chao and Martin 2000; Gerlach et al. 2002; Kellenbach et al. 2003; Buxbaum et al. 2006), it is somewhat surprising that frontal cortex did not show consistent activation in studies of action or artifact words. Several of the included action and artifact studies did report activation in this general region (Martin et al. 1995; Kounios et al. 2003; Tyler, Statamatakis, et al. 2003;,Wheatley et al. 2005; Ruschmeyer et al. 2007), yet these foci did not cluster sufficiently to produce an activation in the ALE analysis. Evidence suggests that inferior frontal and inferior parietal cortices play somewhat different roles in action processing, with the parietal system more closely associated with knowledge of specific object-related actions (Buxbaum 2001; Buxbaum et al. 2005; Creem-Regehr and Lee 2005; Fogassi et al. 2005). Consistent with this distinction, patients with inferior parietal lesions may have impaired recognition of object-related pantomimes performed by others (“representational ideomotor apraxia”), whereas patients with inferior frontal lesions apparently do not (Varney and Damasio 1987; Rothi et al. 1991; Buxbaum et al. 2005). Thus, it is possible that action and artifact words, which in any case do not seem to activate motor representations as readily as pictures do (Rumiati and Humphreys 1998), engage inferior frontal systems only weakly compared with inferior parietal areas concerned with object-related action knowledge.

Some theorists have emphasized a distinction between perceptual (“image-based”) and verbal representations in semantic memory (Paivio 1986), exemplified by the contrast between concrete and abstract words. A number of functional imaging studies have examined this distinction (see Binder 2007 for a review). Thirteen studies included in the present meta-analysis showed areas of stronger activation for concrete compared with abstract words, with overlap in bilateral AG, left mid-fusiform gyrus, left DMPFC, and left posterior cingulate cortex (Fig. 6). This pattern is very similar to the network observed in the general semantic meta-analysis (Fig. 4). Eight studies showed areas of stronger activation for verbal compared with perceptual knowledge. These included comparisons between abstract and concrete words, abstract and concrete stories, and encyclopedic (i.e., verbal factual) versus perceptual knowledge. Abstract concepts are generally more difficult to process than concrete concepts, and several other studies had to be excluded from the meta-analysis because of this confound. Areas associated with verbal semantic processing included the left IFG (mainly pars orbitalis) and left anterior STS. These dissociations support a distinction between perceptually based knowledge, stored in heteromodal association areas such as the AG and ventral temporal lobe, and verbally encoded knowledge, which places greater demands on left anterior perisylvian regions. This dissociation is supported by a range of neuropsychological studies showing relative impairment of abstract word processing in patients with perisylvian lesions (Goodglass et al. 1969; Coltheart et al. 1980; Roeltgen et al. 1983; Katz and Goodglass 1990; Franklin et al. 1995) and relative impairment of concrete word processing in patients with extrasylvian (mainly ventral temporal lobe) lesions (Warrington 1975, 1981; Warrington and Shallice 1984; Breedin et al. 1995; Marshall et al. 1998).

Semantic Processing and Autobiographical Memory Retrieval

The semantic system identified here is virtually identical to the large-scale network identified in recent studies of autobiographical memory retrieval (Maguire 2001; Svoboda et al. 2006). (In their comprehensive review of the autobiographical memory literature, Svoboda et al. [2006] refer to the AG as “temporoparietal junction,” though all of the foci they report in this region are in the inferior parietal lobe.) There are cogent theoretical and empirical reasons to distinguish general semantic from autobiographical memory (Tulving 1972; De Renzi et al. 1987; Yasuda et al. 1997), yet this nearly complete overlap observed in functional imaging studies is striking. Autobiographical memory refers to knowledge about one's own past, including both remembered events, known as episodic autobiographical memory (e.g., “I remember playing tennis last weekend”), and static facts about the self, known as semantic autobiographical memory (e.g., “I like to play tennis”). Most autobiographical memory begins as detailed knowledge about recently experienced events (i.e., episodic memories). With time, these specific memories lose their perceptual detail and come to more closely resemble semantic knowledge (Johnson et al. 1988; Levine et al. 2002; Addis et al. 2004).

There are several reasons to expect overlap between the general semantic and autobiographical memory retrieval systems. First, semantic autobiographical memories, though they differ from general semantic knowledge in referring to the self, are essentially learned facts and therefore might be supported by the same system that stores and retrieves other learned facts. Second, several theorists have proposed that retrieval of general concepts, such as particular temporal and spatial locations, people, objects, and emotions, is an early processing stage in the retrieval of autobiographical memories and serves to cue the retrieval of these more personal memories (Barsalou 1998; Conway and Pleydell-Pearce 2000). Finally, it is worth emphasizing the perhaps obvious point that autobiographical memories are necessarily composed of concepts and that there could be no retrieval of an autobiographical memory without retrieval of concepts. To recall, for example, that “I played tennis last weekend” logically entails retrieval of the concepts “tennis,” “play,” and “weekend.” Thus, the essential distinction between general semantic retrieval and autobiographical memory retrieval lies in the self-referential aspect of the latter, which may be a relatively minor component of the overall process, at least from a neural standpoint.

A few neuroimaging studies have directly compared autobiographical and general semantic retrieval and reported greater activation in the semantic network during the autobiographical condition, particularly in the medial prefrontal cortex, AG, and posterior cingulate region (Graham et al. 2003; Maguire and Frith 2003; Addis et al. 2004; Levine et al. 2004). Rather than indicating a specialization for autobiographical memory processing in these regions, we believe these data reflect the fact that semantic autobiographical memories are typically more perceptually vivid, detailed, contextualized, and emotionally meaningful than general semantic memories. Thus, we propose that semantic autobiographical and general semantic memory processing are supported by largely identical neural networks, but that retrieval of richer and more detailed memories (such as autobiographical memories) engages this network to a greater degree than retrieval of less detailed knowledge.

Semantic Processing and the “Default Network”

As illustrated in Figure 8, the semantic system identified here is also strikingly similar to the human brain “default network” thought to be active during the conscious resting state (Binder et al. 1999; Raichle et al. 2001). This network was originally defined as a set of brain areas that consistently show “task-induced deactivation” in functional imaging studies (Shulman et al. 1997; Binder et al. 1999; Mazoyer et al. 2001; Raichle et al. 2001; McKiernan et al. 2003). Task-induced deactivation refers to decreases in blood flow or BOLD signal during effortful tasks compared with more passive states such as resting, fixation, and passive sensory stimulation.

Comparison of the left-hemisphere general semantic network indicated in the present ALE meta-analysis (top) and the “default network” (bottom). The latter map represents brain areas that showed task-induced deactivation during performance of a tone discrimination task, that is, higher BOLD signal during a conscious resting baseline compared with the tone task (see Binder et al. 2008 for details). In both types of studies, effects are observed in the AG, posterior cingulate gyrus, DMPFC, VMPFC, ventral temporal lobe, anterior MTG, and ventral IFG. Although effects are stronger in the left hemisphere for both kinds of studies, task-induced deactivation is typically more symmetrical in posterior cingulate and medial prefrontal regions (Shulman et al. 1997; Binder et al. 1999; Mazoyer et al. 2001; Raichle et al. 2001; McKiernan et al. 2003).
Figure 8.

Comparison of the left-hemisphere general semantic network indicated in the present ALE meta-analysis (top) and the “default network” (bottom). The latter map represents brain areas that showed task-induced deactivation during performance of a tone discrimination task, that is, higher BOLD signal during a conscious resting baseline compared with the tone task (see Binder et al. 2008 for details). In both types of studies, effects are observed in the AG, posterior cingulate gyrus, DMPFC, VMPFC, ventral temporal lobe, anterior MTG, and ventral IFG. Although effects are stronger in the left hemisphere for both kinds of studies, task-induced deactivation is typically more symmetrical in posterior cingulate and medial prefrontal regions (Shulman et al. 1997; Binder et al. 1999; Mazoyer et al. 2001; Raichle et al. 2001; McKiernan et al. 2003).

There is now a general consensus that task-induced deactivation occurs because certain types of neural processes active during passive states are “interrupted” when subjects are engaged in effortful tasks (Binder et al. 1999; Raichle 2006), though the precise nature of these default neural processes remains a topic of debate. Evidence suggests that high levels of ongoing “spontaneous” neural activity are necessary for maximizing responsiveness of the cortex to changes in input (Ho and Destexhe 2000; Salinas and Sejnowski 2001; Chance et al. 2002), and this ongoing activity seems to account for much of the high resting metabolic needs of the cortex (Attwell and Laughlin 2001). The mere fact that there are high levels of neural activity in the resting state does not explain, however, why this activity decreases in particular brain regions during active task states. Such an explanation would seem to depend on an account of resting state activity in terms of the cognitive and affective processes in which subjects preferentially engage during passive states, such as episodic memory encoding and retrieval, monitoring and evaluating the internal and external environment, visual imagery, emotion processing, and working memory (Andreasen et al. 1995; Shulman et al. 1997; Mazoyer et al. 2001; Raichle et al. 2001; Simpson et al. 2001; Stark and Squire 2001).

Binder et al. (1999) proposed that semantic processing constitutes a large component of the cognitive activity occurring during passive states. This proposal was based first on phenomenological considerations. The everyday experience of spontaneous thoughts, mental narratives, and imagery that James referred to as the “stream of consciousness” (James 1890) is ubiquitous and undeniable. Participants in controlled laboratory conditions reliably report such “task-unrelated thinking,” the content of which includes concepts, emotions, and images (Antrobus et al. 1966; Antrobus 1968; Horowitz 1975; Pope and Singer 1976; Teasdale et al. 1993, 1995; Giambra 1995; McKiernan et al. 2003). Performance of effortful perceptual and short-term memory tasks reliably suppresses task-unrelated thoughts, suggesting a direct competition between exogenous and endogenous signals for attentional and executive resources. “Interrupting the stream of consciousness” thus provides a straightforward explanation for task-induced deactivation.

The second consideration emphasized by Binder et al. was the potential adaptive advantage of systems that allow “ongoing” retrieval of conceptual knowledge. In contrast to the sensory, spatial attention, and motor processes engaged when an external stimulus requires a response, ongoing conceptual processes operate primarily on internal stores of knowledge built from past experience. They are not random or “spontaneous” but rather play a profoundly important role in human behavior. Their purpose is to “make sense” of prior experience, solve problems that require computation over long periods of time, and create effective plans governing behavior in the future. This uniquely human capability to perform high-level computations “off-line” is surely the principal explanation for our ability to adapt, create culture, and invent technology.

The final evidence offered by Binder et al. was an fMRI study in which participants were scanned while resting, while performing a perceptual task with no semantic content, and while making semantic decisions about words. Relative to the resting state, the perceptual task produced deactivation in the usual default network (AG, posterior cingulate gyrus, VMPFC, and DMPFC). Critically, however, the semantic decision task did not deactivate these regions. Finally, the same regions were activated when contrasting the semantic decision task with a phonological task that controlled for sensory and executive processes. These results show not only that the semantic network (as defined by the semantic–phonological task contrast) is virtually identical to the default state network, but also that task-induced deactivation in these regions can be modulated by task content. Deactivation occurs when the task or stimuli make little or no demands on semantic processing. When the task itself engages the semantic system, however, deactivation does not occur. This pattern indicates that the default network itself is engaged in semantic processing, both during passive states and when presented with a semantic task. This pattern has since been replicated in a number of studies comparing word tasks, pseudoword tasks, and passive or resting conditions. Pseudowords reliably deactivate the default network relative to the resting state, whereas words do not (Baumgaertner et al. 2002; Henson et al. 2002; Mechelli et al. 2003; Rissman et al. 2003; Ischebeck et al. 2004; Binder, Medler, et al. 2005; Xiao et al. 2005; Humphries et al. 2007). This result is fully consistent with the model just proposed, because pseudowords have little meaning and thus do not engage the semantic system.

Conceptual versus Perceptual Processing

The observation that semantic processing engages a network of heteromodal association areas distinct from modal sensory and motor systems provides support for the traditional distinction between conceptual and perceptual processes (Fodor 1983). Under this view, conceptual processes operate on “internal” sources of information, such as semantic and episodic memory, which can be retrieved and manipulated at any time, independent of ongoing external events. In contrast, perceptual processes operate on “external” information derived from immediate, ongoing sensory and motor processes that coordinate interactions with the external environment. Many tasks, of course, require simultaneous processing of both internal and external information, including, for example, any behavior involving recognition of a familiar stimulus such as a word or picture. In such instances, external sensory information is given meaning by activation of associated internal information (Neisser 1967; Aurell 1979).

Functional imaging evidence for such a distinction is illustrated dramatically in Figure 9, which shows areas identified with semantic processing in the present meta-analysis in red. Yellow areas in the figure were activated during a task in which subjects read pseudowords aloud, thereby engaging ventral visual form perception, dorsal visual and spatial attention, motor articulation, and auditory perceptual systems bilaterally (Binder, Medler, et al. 2005). Apart from a few areas of overlap in the anterior ventral visual stream, STS, and IFG, these large-scale networks are largely distinct and complementary, together covering much of the cortical surface. Similar imaging evidence supporting a general distinction between large-scale “intrinsic” and “extrinsic” networks has been reported recently by other researchers (Fox et al. 2005; Fransson 2005; Golland et al. 2007).

Composite map of complementary human brain networks for processing internal and external sources of information. Red areas indicate the general semantic network identified in the current meta-analysis. Yellow indicates areas activated in 24 healthy adults during oral reading of visually presented pseudowords (pronounceable but meaningless letter strings) compared with a resting baseline (Binder, Medler, et al. 2005). The latter task activates unimodal visual and auditory areas bilaterally, dorsal attention systems in the IPS and FEF bilaterally, and bilateral motor, premotor, dorsal anterior cingulate, and dorsolateral prefrontal systems involved in response production. Overlap between these 2 major networks is minimal.
Figure 9.

Composite map of complementary human brain networks for processing internal and external sources of information. Red areas indicate the general semantic network identified in the current meta-analysis. Yellow indicates areas activated in 24 healthy adults during oral reading of visually presented pseudowords (pronounceable but meaningless letter strings) compared with a resting baseline (Binder, Medler, et al. 2005). The latter task activates unimodal visual and auditory areas bilaterally, dorsal attention systems in the IPS and FEF bilaterally, and bilateral motor, premotor, dorsal anterior cingulate, and dorsolateral prefrontal systems involved in response production. Overlap between these 2 major networks is minimal.

Although these observations confirm a general distinction between conceptual and perceptual systems in the brain, other evidence suggests that this distinction is not absolute (Gallese and Lakoff 2005). For example, several studies have shown involvement of primary motor and premotor cortex in the comprehension of action verbs (Hauk et al. 2004; Pulvermuller et al. 2005; Tettamanti et al. 2005). Similarly, there is evidence that high-level visual cortex participates in the processing of object nouns (Martin et al. 1995; James and Gauthier 2003; Kan et al. 2003; Simmons et al. 2007). Word-related activation of these motor and sensory areas is likely to be somewhat subtle compared with activation of heteromodal conceptual regions and to depend to a greater extent on the specific sensorimotor attributes of the word concept. Furthermore, there are as yet few published studies that have focused on such specific attributes. Thus, the present meta-analysis may underrepresent the involvement of sensory and motor systems in comprehension of word stimuli. Defining the extent of this involvement is an important topic for future research.

Comparison with Previous Large-Scale Reviews of Semantic Processing

The current results differ somewhat from conclusions reached in previous reviews. Cabeza and Nyberg (2000) reviewed functional imaging studies published prior to 1999. From an initial sample of 275 studies in a range of cognitive domains, they found 31 involving semantic memory retrieval tasks. Activation foci from these studies clustered mainly in the left IFG, with a few additional foci in the left MTG. The authors provided a brief description of the task contrasts used in each study and drew attention to the importance of control tasks, but made no attempt to exclude studies with word-form or task difficulty confounds. Of these 31 studies, only 4 passed our criteria for inclusion, with the remainder excluded mainly for phonological or task difficulty confounds.

Vigneau et al. (2006) collected 65 studies published prior to 2005 that concerned semantic processing. The authors did not report how the studies were identified but indicated that they excluded contrasts that used visual fixation or resting controls. Both object and word studies were included. No attempt was made to identify phonological or task difficulty confounds. Only activation peaks in the lateral temporal, lateral frontal, and inferior parietal lobes were included in the analysis. Foci were found throughout these regions, including the STG, MTG, IFG, and premotor cortex. The findings of Vigneau et al. thus differ from the current results in showing numerous foci in the STG (due to inclusion of studies comparing speech with non–speech sounds) and in the posterior IFG and premotor cortex (likely due to phonological and working memory confounds), and in the a priori exclusion of data from ventral temporal, dorsomedial prefrontal, posterior cingulate, and ventromedial frontal areas. Of the 65 studies selected by Vigneau et al., only 17 passed our criteria for inclusion; most were excluded because of task confounds or use of object stimuli. Another 53 studies that passed our criteria and were published prior to 2005 were not identified by Vigneau et al.

Future meta-analyses of cognitive imaging studies would benefit from the establishment of a more formal ontological system for defining the cognitive processes represented by an “activation.” The aim of such meta-analyses is to identify the neural correlates of a specific cognitive process or related set of processes, but this enterprise cannot logically succeed without an objective means of defining the cognitive components represented by an activation. The “objects” in such an ontology would correspond to particular experimental conditions (i.e., cognitive processing states), specified by sets of operationally-defined stimulus and task attributes. Contrasts between experimental conditions would constitute “relations” defining the cognitive processes that differ between conditions. The system would rest on a set of agreed-upon axioms concerning stimuli, tasks, and their attributes (e.g., “words are familiar symbols,” “familiar symbols have associations,” “associations are stored in semantic memory,” etc.). The interpretive power of such an ontology would be invaluable not only for retrospective meta-analysis but also for designing and interpreting future studies within a common theoretical framework.

Conclusions

The neural systems specialized for storage and retrieval of semantic knowledge are widespread and occupy a large proportion of the cortex in the human brain. The areas implicated in these processes can be grouped into 3 broad categories: posterior heteromodal association cortex (AG, MTG, and fusiform gyrus), specific subregions of heteromodal prefrontal cortex (dorsal, ventromedial, and inferior prefrontal cortex), and medial paralimbic regions with strong connections to the hippocampal formation (parahippocampus and posterior cingulate gyrus). The widespread involvement of heteromodal cortex is notable in that these regions are greatly expanded in the human relative to the nonhuman primate brain. This evolutionary expansion of neural systems supporting conceptual processing probably accounts for uniquely human capacities to use language productively, plan the future, solve problems, and create cultural and technological artifacts, all of which depend on the fluid and efficient retrieval and manipulation of conceptual knowledge.

Funding

National Institutes of Health (grants R01 NS33576, R01 NS35929, R01 DC006287, R03 DC008416, T32 MH019992, and F32 HD056767).

The authors thank B. Douglas Ward for invaluable technical assistance. Conflict of Interest: None declared.

Appendix 1: List of Included Studies

  1. Assaf M, Calhoun VD, Kuzu CH, Kraut MA, Rivkin PR, Hart J, et al. Neural correlates of the object-recall process in semantic memory. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 2006; 147: 115–126.

  2. Baumgaertner A, Buccino G, Lange R, McNamara A, Binkofski F. Polymodal conceptual processing of human biological actions in the left inferior frontal lobe. European Journal of Neuroscience 2007; 25: 881–889.

  3. Baumgaertner A, Weiller C, Buchel C. Event-related fMRI reveals cortical sites involved in contextual sentence integration. Neuroimage 2002; 16: 736–745.

  4. Bedny M, Thompson-Schill SL. Neuroanatomically separable effects of imageability and grammatical class during single-word comprehension. Brain and Language 2006; 98: 127–139.

  5. Binder JR, Frost JA, Hammeke TA, Bellgowan PSF, Rao SM, Cox RW. Conceptual processing during the conscious resting state: a functional MRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 1999; 11: 80–93.

  6. Binder JR, McKiernan KA, Parsons M, Westbury CF, Possing ET, Kaufman JN, et al. Neural correlates of lexical access during visual word recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2003; 15: 372–393.

  7. Binder JR, Medler DA, Desai R, Conant LL, Liebenthal E. Some neurophysiological constraints on models of word naming. Neuroimage 2005a; 27: 677–693.

  8. Binder JR, Westbury CF, Possing ET, McKiernan KA, Medler DA. Distinct brain systems for processing concrete and abstract concepts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2005b; 17: 905–917.

  9. Blaxton TA, Bookheimer SY, Zeffiro TA, Figlozzi CM, Gaillard WD, Theodore WH. Functional mapping of human memory using PET: comparisons of conceptual and perceptual tasks. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 1996; 50: 42–56.

  10. Booth JR, Lu D, Burman DD, Chou TL, Jin Z, Peng DL, et al. Specialization of phonological and semantic processing in Chinese word reading. Brain Research 2006; 1071: 197–207.

  11. Boronat CB, Buxbaum LJ, Coslett HB, Tang K, Saffran EM, Kimberg DY, et al. Distinctions between manipulation and function knowledge of objects: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging. Cogn Brain Research 2005; 23: 361–373.

  12. Cai C, Kochiyama T, Osaka K, Wu J. Lexical/semantic processing in dorsal left inferior frontal gyrus. Neuroreport 2007; 18: 1147–1151.

  13. Cappa SF, Perani D, Schnur T, Tettamanti M, Fazio F. The effects of semantic category and knowledge type on lexical-semantic access: A PET study. Neuroimage 1998; 8: 350–359.

  14. Cato MA, Crosson B, Gokcay D, Soltysik D, Wierenga C, Gopinath K, et al. Processing words with emotional connotation: an FMRI study of time course and laterality in rostral frontal and retrosplenial cortices. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2004; 16: 167–177.

  15. Clark D, Wagner AD. Assembling and encoding word representations: fMRI subsequent memory effects implicate a role for phonological control. Neuropsychologia 2003; 41: 304–317.

  16. Collette F, Majerus S, Van der Linden M, Dabe P, Degueldre C, Delfiore G, et al. Contribution of lexico-semantic processes to verbal short-term memory tasks: A PET activation study. Memory 2001; 9: 249–259.

  17. Copland DA, de Zubicaray GI, McMahon K, Eastburn M. Neural correlates of semantic priming for ambiguous words: An event-related fMRI study. Brain Research 2007; 1131: 163–172.

  18. Craik FIM, Moroz TM, Moscovitch M, Stuss DT, Winocur G, Tulving E, et al. In search of the self: A positron emission tomography study. Psychological Science 1999; 10: 26–34.

  19. Crosson B, Radonovich K, Sadek JR, Gokcay D, Bauer RM, Fischler IS, et al. Left-hemisphere processing of emotional connotation during word generation. Neuroreport 1999; 10: 2449–2455.

  20. D'Arcy RCN, Ryner L, Richter W, Service E, Connolly JF. The fan effect in fMRI: Left hemisphere specialization in verbal working memory. Neuroreport 2004; 15: 1851–1855.

  21. Dapretto M, Bookheimer SY. Form and content: Dissociating syntax and semantics in sentence comprehension. Neuron 1999; 24: 427–432.

  22. Daselaar SM, Veltman DJ, Rombouts ARB, Raaijmakers JGW, Lazeron RHC, Jonker C. Medial temporal lobe activity during semantic classification using a flexible fMRI design. Behavioural Brain Research 2002; 136: 399–404.

  23. Davis MH, Meunier F, Marslen-Wilson WD. Neural responses to morphological, syntactic, and semantic properties of single words: An fMRI study. Brain and Language 2004; 89: 439–449.

  24. Démonet J-F, Chollet F, Ramsay S, Cardebat D, Nespoulous J-L, Wise R, et al. The anatomy of phonological and semantic processing in normal subjects. Brain 1992; 115: 1753–1768.

  25. Devlin JT, Matthews PM, Rushworth MFS. Semantic processing in the left inferior prefrontal cortex: A combined functional magnetic resonance imaging and transcranial magnetic stimulation study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2003; 15: 71–84.

  26. Devlin JT, Russell RP, Davis MH, Price CJ, Moss HE, Jalal Fadili M, et al. Is there an anatomical basis for category-specificity? Semantic memory studies with PET and fMRI. Neuropsychologia 2002; 40: 54–75.

  27. Ebisch SJH, Babiloni C, Del Gratta C, Ferretti A, Perrucci MG, Caulo M, et al. Human neural systems for conceptual knowledge of proper object use: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Cerebral Cortex 2007; 17: 2744–2751.

  28. Eschen A, Freeman J, Dietrich T, Martin M, Ellis J, Martin E, et al. Motor brain regions are involved in the encoding of delayed intentions: A fMRI study. International Journal of Psychophysiology 2007; 64: 259–268.

  29. Fiebach CJ, Friederici AD. Processing concrete words: fMRI evidence against a specific right-hemisphere involvement. Neuropsychologia 2003; 42: 62–70.

  30. Fiebach CJ, Friederici AD, Muller K, von Cramon DY. FMRI evidence for dual routes to the mental lexicon in visual word recognition. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2002; 14: 11–23.

  31. Fletcher PC, Happé F, Frith U, Baker SC, Dolan RJ, Frackowiak RSJ, et al. Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension. Cognition 1995; 57: 109–128.

  32. Fliessbach K, Wesi S, Klaver P, Elger CE, Weber B. The effect of word concreteness on recognition memory. Neuroimage 2006; 32: 1413–1421.

  33. Giesbrecht B, Camblin CC, Swaab TY. Separable effects of semantic priming and imageability on word processing in human cortex. Cerebral Cortex 2004; 14: 521–529.

  34. Giraud AL, Price CJ. The constraints functional neuroimaging places on classical models of auditory word processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2001; 13: 754–765.

  35. Gitelman DR, Nobre AC, Sonty S, Parrish TB, Mesulam MM. Language network specializations: An analysis with parallel task designs and functional magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroimage 2005; 26: 975–985.

  36. Goldberg RF, Perfetti CA, Schneider W. Distinct and common cortical activations for multimodal semantic categories. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 2006a; 6: 214–222.

  37. Goldberg RF, Perfetti CA, Schneider W. Perceptual knowledge retrieval activates sensory brain regions. Journal of Neuroscience 2006b; 26: 4917–4921.

  38. Gorno-Tempini ML, Price CJ, Josephs O, Vandenberghe R, Cappa SF, Kapur N, et al. The neural systems sustaining face and proper-name processing. Brain 1998; 121: 2103–2118.

  39. Gourovitch ML, Kirkby BS, Goldberg TE, Weinberger DR, Gold JM, Esposito G, et al. A comparison of rCBF patterns during letter and semantic fluency. Neuropsychology 2000; 14: 353–360.

  40. Grossman M, Koenig P, DeVita C, Glosser G, Alsop D, Detre J, et al. The neural basis for category-specific knowledge: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 2002a; 15: 936–948.

  41. Grossman M, Koenig P, DeVita C, Glosser G, Alsop D, Detre J, et al. Neural representation of verb meaning: An fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping 2002b; 15: 124–134.

  42. Hagoort P, Indefrey P, Brown C, Herzog H, Steinmetz H, Seitz RJ. The neural circuitry involved in the reading of German words and pseudowords: A PET study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 1999; 11: 383–398.

  43. Henke K, Weber B, Kneifel S, Wieser HG, Buck A. Human hippocampus associates information in memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 1999; 96: 5884–5889.

  44. Henson RNA. Repetition effects for words and nonwords as indexed by event-related fMRI: a preliminary study. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 2001; 42: 179–186.

  45. Henson RNA, Price CJ, Rugg MD, Turner R, Friston KJ. Detecting latency differences in event-related BOLD responses: Application to words versus nonwords and initial versus repeated face presentations. Neuroimage 2002; 15: 83–97.

  46. Herbster AN, Mintun MA, Nebes RD, Becker JT. Regional cerebral blood flow during word and nonword reading. Human Brain Mapping 1997; 5: 84–92.

  47. Homae F, Hashimoto R, Nakajima K, Miyashita Y, Sakai KL. From perception to sentence comprehension: the convergence of auditory and visual information of language in the left inferior frontal cortex. Neuroimage 2002; 16: 883–900.

  48. Homae F, Yahata N, Sakai KL. Selective enhancement of functional connectivity in the left prefrontal cortex during sentence processing. Neuroimage 2003; 20: 578–586.

  49. Humphries C, Binder JR, Medler DA, Liebenthal E. Syntactic and semantic modulation of neural activity during auditory sentence comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2006; 18: 665–679.

  50. Ischebeck A, Indefrey P, Usui N, Nose I, Hellwig F, Taira M. Reading in a regular orthography: An fMRI study investigating the role of visual familiarity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2004; 16: 727–741.

  51. Jessen F, Heun R, Erb M, Granath DO, Klose U, Papassotiropoulos A, et al. The concreteness effect: Evidence for dual-coding and context availability. Brain and Language 2000; 74: 103–112.

  52. Joubert SA, Beauregard M, Walter N, Bourgouin P, Beaudoin G, Leroux J-M, et al. Neural correlates of lexical and sublexical processes in reading. Brain and Language 2004; 89: 9–20.

  53. Kellenbach ML, Brett M, Patterson K. Large, colourful or noisy? Attribute- and modality-specific activations during retrieval of perceptual attribute knowledge. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience 2001; 1: 207–221.

  54. Kelley WM, Macrae CN, Wyland CL, Caglar S, Inati S, Heatherton TF. Finding the self? An event-related fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2002; 14: 785–794.

  55. Kosslyn SM, Alpert NM, Thompson WL. Identifying objects at different levels of hierarchy: A positron emission tomography study. Human Brain Mapping 1995; 3: 107–132.

  56. Kotz SA, Cappa SF, von Cramon DY, Friederici AD. Modulation of the lexical-semantic network by auditory semantic priming: An event-related functional MRI study. Neuroimage 2002; 17: 1761–1772.

  57. Kounios J, Koenig P, Glosser G, DeVita C, Dennis K, Moore P, et al. Category-specific medial temporal lobe activation and the consolidation of semantic memory. Cogn Brain Research 2003; 17: 484–494.

  58. Kuchinke L, Jacobs AM, Grubich C, Vo MLH, Conrad M, Herrmann M. Incidental effects of emotional valence in single word processing: An fMRI study. Neuroimage 2005; 28: 1022–1032.

  59. Kuperberg GR, Lakshmanan BM, Caplan DN, Holcomb PJ. Making sense of discourse: An fMRI study of causal inferencing across sentences. Neuroimage 2006; 33: 343–361.

  60. Kuperberg GR, McGuire PK, Bullmore ET, Brammer MJ, Rabe-Hesketh S, Wright I, et al. Common and distinct neural substrates for pragmatic, semantic, and syntactic processing of spoken sentences: an fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2000; 12: 321–341.

  61. Laine M, Rinne JO, Hiltunen J, Kaasinen V, Sipila H. Different brain activation patterns during production of animals versus artefacts: a PET activation study on category-specific processing. Cogn Brain Research 2002; 13: 95–99.

  62. Lee AC, Graham KS, Simons JS, Hodges JR, Owen AM, Patterson K. Regional brain activations differ for semantic features but not categories. Neuroreport 2002a; 13: 1497–1501.

  63. Lee ACH, Robbins TW, Graham KS, Owen AM. “Pray or prey?” Dissociation of semantic memory retrieval from epiesodic memory processes using positron emission tomography and a novel homophone task. Neuroimage 2002b; 16: 724–735.

  64. Lee SS, Dapretto M. Metaphorical vs. literal word meanings: fMRI evidence against a selective role of the right hemisphere. Neuroimage 2006; 29: 536–544.

  65. Liu C, Hue C, Chen C, Chuang K, Liang K, Wang Y, et al. Dissociated roles of the middle frontal gyri in the processing of Chinese characters. Neuroreport 2006; 17: 1397–1401.

  66. Luo J, Niki K. Role of medial temporal lobe in extensive retrieval of task-related knowledge. Hippocampus 2002; 12: 487–494.

  67. Majerus S, Van der Linden M, Collette F, Laureys S, Poncelet M, Degueldre C, et al. Modulation of brain activity during phonological familiarization. Brain and Language 2005; 92: 320–331.

  68. Maril A, Wagner AD, Schacter DL. On the tip of the tongue: An event-related fMRI study of semantic retrieval failure and cognitive conflict. Neuron 2001; 31: 653–660.

  69. Martin A, Haxby JV, Lalonde FM, Wiggs CL, Ungerleider LG. Discrete cortical regions associated with knowledge of color and knowledge of action. Science 1995; 270: 102–105.

  70. Mashal N, Faust M, Hendler T, Jung-Beeman M. An fMRI investigation of the neural correlates underlying the processing of novel metaphoric expressions. Brain and Language 2007; 100: 115–126.

  71. Mechelli A, Josephs O, Lambon Ralph MA, McClelland JL, Price CJ. Dissociating stimulus-driven semantic and phonological effect during reading and naming. Human Brain Mapping 2007; 28: 205–217.

  72. Meyer M, Alter K, Friederici AD, Lohmann G, von Cramon DY. FMRI reveals brain regions mediating slow prosodic modulations in spoken sentences. Human Brain Mapping 2002; 17: 73–88.

  73. Miceli G, Turriziani P, Caltagirone C, Capasso R, Tomaiuolo F, Caramazza A. The neural correlates of grammatical gender: An fMRI investigation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2002; 14: 618–628.

  74. Mummery CJ, Patterson K, Hodges JR, Price CJ. Functional neuroanatomy of the semantic system: divisible by what? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 1998; 10: 766–777.

  75. Mummery CJ, Patterson K, Hodges JR, Wise RJS. Generating ‘tiger’ as an animal name or a word beginning with T: differences in brain activation. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 1996; 263: 989–995.

  76. Nakic M, Smith BW, Busis S, Vythilingam M, Blair RJR. The impact of affect and frequency on lexical decision: The role of the amygdala and inferior frontal cortex. Neuroimage 2006; 31: 1752–1761.

  77. Nichelli P, Grafman J, Pietrini P, Clark K, Lee KY, Miletich R. Where the brain appreciates the moral of a story. Neuroreport 1995; 6: 2309–2313.

  78. Noppeney U, Friston KJ, Price CJ. Effects of visual deprivation on the organization of the semantic system. Brain 2003; 126: 1620–1627.

  79. Noppeney U, Josephs O, Kiebel S, Friston KJ, Price CJ. Action selectivity in parietal and temporal cortex. Cogn Brain Research 2005; 25: 641–649.

  80. Noppeney U, Price CJ. Retrieval of visual, auditory, and abstract semantics. Neuroimage 2002; 15: 917–926.

  81. Noppeney U, Price CJ. Functional imaging of the semantic system: Retrieval of sensory-experienced and verbally learned knowledge. Brain and Language 2003; 84: 120–133.

  82. Noppeney U, Price CJ. Retrieval of abstract semantics. Neuroimage 2004; 22: 164–170.

  83. Obleser J, Wise RJS, Dresner MA, Scott SK. Functional integration across brain regions improves speech perception under adverse listening conditions. Journal of Neuroscience 2007; 27: 2283–2289.

  84. Orfanidou E, Marslen-Wilson WD, Davis MH. Neural response suppression predicts repetition priming of spoken words and pseudowords. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2006; 18: 1237–1252.

  85. Otten LJ, Rugg MD. Task-dependency of the neural correlates of episodic encoding as measured by fMRI. Cerebral Cortex 2001; 11: 1150–1160.

  86. Paulesu E, McCrory E, Fazio F, Menoncello L, Brunswick N, Cappa SF, et al. A cultural effect on brain function. Nature Neuroscience 2000; 3: 91–96.

  87. Perani D, Dehaene S, Grassi F, Cohen L, Cappa SF, Dupoux E, et al. Brain processing of native and foreign languages. Neuroreport 1996; 7: 2439–2444.

  88. Perani D, Schnur T, Tettamanti M, Gorno-Tempini M, Cappa SF, Fazio F. Word and picture matching: A PET study of semantic category effects. Neuropsychologia 1999; 37: 293–306.

  89. Pilgrim LK, Fadili J, Fletcher P, Tyler LK. Overcoming confounds of stimulus blocking: An event-related fMRI design of semantic processing. Neuroimage 2002; 16: 713–723.

  90. Price CJ, Moore CJ, Humphreys GW, Wise RJS. Segregating semantic from phonological processes during reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 1997; 9: 727–733.

  91. Price CJ, Wise RSJ, Frackowiak RSJ. Demonstrating the implicit processing of visually presented words and pseudowords. Cerebral Cortex 1996; 6: 62–70.

  92. Prince SE, Tsukiura T, Cabeza R. Distinguishing the neural correlates of episodic memory encoding and semantic memory retrieval. Psychological Science 2007; 18: 144–151.

  93. Rajah MN, McIntosh AM. Overlap in the functional neural systems involved in semantic and episodic memory retrieval. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2005; 17: 470–482.

  94. Raposo A, Moss HE, Stamatakis EA, Tyler LK. Repetition suppression and semantic enhancement: An investigation of the neural correlates of priming. Neuropsychologia 2006; 44: 2284–2295.

  95. Rissman J, Eliassen JC, Blumstein SE. An event-related fMRI investigation of implicit semantic priming. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2003; 15: 1160–1175.

  96. Robertson DA, Gernsbacher MA, Guidotti SJ, Robertson RRW, Irwin W, Mock BJ, et al. Functional neuroanatomy of the cognitive process of mapping during discourse comprehension. Psychological Science 2000; 11: 255–260.

  97. Rogers TT, Hocking J, Noppeney U, Mechelli A, Gorno-Tempini ML, Patterson K, et al. Anterior temporal cortex and semantic memory: reconciling findings from neuropsychology and functional imaging. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience 2006; 6: 201–213.

  98. Roskies AL, Fiez JA, Balota DA, Raichle ME, Petersen SE. Task-dependent modulation of regions in the left inferior frontal cortex during semantic processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2001; 13: 829–843.

  99. Ruschmeyer S-A, Brass M, Friederici AD. Comprehending prehending: Neural correlates of processing verbs with motor stems. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2007; 19: 855–865.

  100. Sabsevitz DS, Medler DA, Seidenberg M, Binder JR. Modulation of the semantic system by word imageability. Neuroimage 2005; 27: 188–200.

  101. Satpute AB, Fenker DB, Waldmann MR, Tabibnia G, Holyoak KJ, Lieberman MD. An fMRI study of causal judgments. European Journal of Neuroscience 2005; 22: 1233–1238.

  102. Scott SK, Leff AP, Wise RJS. Going beyond the information given: a neural system supporting semantic interpretation. Neuroimage 2003; 19: 870–876.

  103. Stringaris AK, Medford NC, Giampietro V, Brammer MJ, David AS. Deriving meaning: Distinct neural mechanisms for metaphoric, literal, and non-meaningful sentences. Brain and Language 2007; 100: 150–162.

  104. Stringaris AK, Medford NC, Giora R, Giampietro VC, Brammer MJ, David AS. How metaphors influence semantic relatedness judgments: The role of the right frontal cortex. Neuroimage 2006; 33: 784–793.

  105. Sugiura M, Sassa Y, Watanabe J, Akitsuki Y, Maeda Y, Matsue Y, et al. Cortical mechanisms of person representation: recognition of famous and personally familiar names. Neuroimage 2006; 31: 853–860.

  106. Thioux M, Pesenti M, Costes N, De Volder A, Seron X. Task-independent semantic activation for numbers and animals. Cogn Brain Research 2005; 24: 284–290.

  107. Thompson CK, Bonakdarpour B, Fix SC, Blumenfeld HK, Parrish TB, Gitelman DR, et al. Neural correlates of verb argument structure processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2007; 19: 1753–1767.

  108. Tomasino B, Werner C, Weiss PH, Fink GR. Stimulus properties matter more than perspective: An fMRI study of mental imagery and silent reading of action phrases. Neuroimage 2007; 36: T128–141.

  109. Tyler LK, Russell R, Fadili J, Moss HE. The neural representation of nouns and verbs: PET studies. Brain 2001; 124: 1619–1634.

  110. Tyler LK, Stamatakis EA, Dick E, Bright P, Fletcher P, Moss HE. Objects and their actions: evidence for a neurally distributed semantic system. Neuroimage 2003; 18: 542–557.

  111. Vandenberghe R, Nobre AC, Price CJ. The response of left temporal cortex to sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2002; 14: 550–560.

  112. von Kriegstein K, Eger E, Kleinschmidt A, Giraud AL. Modulation of neural responses to speech by directing attention to voices or verbal content. Cogn Brain Research 2003; 17: 48–55.

  113. Wagner AD, Pare-Blagoev EJ, Clark J, Poldrack RA. Recovering meaning: left prefrontal cortex guides semantic retrieval. Neuron 2001; 31: 329–338.

  114. Wallentin M, Østergaarda S, Lund TE, Østergaard L, Roepstorff A. Concrete spatial language: See what I mean? Brain and Language 2005; 92: 221–233.

  115. Whatmough C, Verret L, Fung D, Chertkow H. Common and contrasting areas of activation for abstract and concrete concepts: An H2 15O PET study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2004; 16: 1211–1226.

  116. Wheatley T, Weisberg J, Beauchamp MS, Martin A. Automatic priming of semantically related words reduces activity in the fusiform gyrus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2005; 17: 1871–1885.

  117. Wise RJS, Howard D, Mummery CJ, Fletcher P, Leff A, Büchel C, et al. Noun imageability and the temporal lobes. Neuropsychologia 2000; 38: 985–994.

  118. Woodard JL, Seidenberg M, Nielson KA, Miller SK, Franczak M, Antuono P, et al. Temporally graded activation of neocortical regions in response to memories of different ages. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 2007; 19: 1–12.

  119. Xiao Z, Zhang JX, Wang X, Wu R, Hu X, Weng X, et al. Differential activity in left inferior frontal gyrus for pseudowords and real words: an event-related fMRI study on auditory lexical decision. Human Brain Mapping 2005; 25: 212–221.

  120. Xu J, Kemeny S, Park G, Frattali C, Braun A. Language in context: emergent features of word, sentence, and narrative comprehension. Neuroimage 2005; 25: 1002–1015.

Appendix 2. Individual Study Details
No.1st AuthorNMethodTypeContrastActivation in brain regions of interest
IPLMTGFG/PHDMPFCIFGVMPFCPC
1Assaf18fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)++++++
2Baumgaertner19fMRISpecAction–Motion(SD)++++
3Baumgaertner9fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++
4Bedny13fMRISpecConc–Abst(SD)++
5Binder30fMRIGenW(SD)–N(PD)++++
6Binder24fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++++++
7Binder24fMRIGenW–N(Read)+++++++
SpecConc–Abst(Read)++++++
8Binder24fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++++++
SpecConc–Abst(LD)+++
9Blaxton14PETGenW(SD)–N(PD)++++++
10Booth13fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)++
11Boronat15fMRISpecAction–Function(SD)+
12Cai15fMRIGenW–N(PD)+
13Cappa13PETGenW(SD)–N(PD)+++
SpecTool–Animal(SD)++
14Cato26fMRISpecEmotion–Neutral(cWG)+++++
15Clark20fMRIGenW–N(PD)+++++
16Collette12PETGenW–N(Rep)+
17Copland14fMRIGenRel–Unrel(LD)+
18Craik8PETGenW(SD)–W(PD)+++++
19Crosson17fMRISpecEmotion–Neutral(cWG)+
20D'Arcy10fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)
21Dapretto8fMRIGenSemSP–SynSP(SD)+
22Daselaar6fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)++++
23Davis12fMRISpecAction–Other(SD)+
24Démonet9PETGenW(SD)–N(PD)++++
25Devlin12fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)+++++
26Devlin26BothSpecArtifact–Natural(SD)
27Ebisch17fMRISpecFunction–Size(SD)++++
28Eschen10fMRISpecAction–Abst(Mem)
29Fiebach12fMRISpecConc–Abst(LD)
30Fiebach11fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++
31Fletcher6PETSpecStory–UnrelSent(SD)++++
SpecThM–Story(SD)+++
32Fliessbach21fMRISpecConc–Abst(Rec)+
33Giesbrecht10fMRISpecConc–Abst(SD)+++
34Giraud12PETGenW–N(Rep)
35Gitelman14fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)++++
36Goldberg13fMRISpecNatural–Other(SD)+++
37Goldberg14fMRISpecColor/Taste/ etc.–Other(SD)++++
38Gorno-Tempini6PETSpecPerson–Object(Match)+
39Gourovitch18PETGenW(SWG)–W(PWG)++++
40Grossman16fMRISpecTool–Animal(SD)+++
41Grossman16fMRISpecMotion–Abst(SD)++
42Hagoort11PETGenW–N(Read)++
43Henke12PETGenW(SD)–W(PD)++
44Henson6fMRIGenW–N(OD)+++
45Henson12fMRIGenW–N(LD)++++
46Herbster10PETGenW–N(cRead)+
47Homae9fMRIGenW/N(LD)–N(Match)+++++
GenSent(SD)–W/N(LD)++++
48Homae10fMRISpecStory(SD)–UnrelSent(LD)+++
49Humphries21fMRIGenHighM–LowM(SD)+++++
GenHighM–N(SD)++++
GenLowM–N(SD)+++
50Ischebeck10fMRIGenW–N(cRead)++++
7GenW–N(LD)++++
51Jessen14fMRISpecConc–Abst(Mem)++++
52Joubert10fMRIGenW–N(cRead)++
53Kellenbach10PETSpecAuditory–Visual(SD)++++
SpecSize–Color(SD)+
54Kelley21fMRISpecSelf–Other(SD)++
55Kosslyn12PETSpecCategory–Other(SD)++
56Kotz13fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++
GenRel–Unrel(LD)+
SpecCategory–Association(LD)++
57Kounios16fMRISpecAnimal–Tool(SD)++
SpecAnimal–Abst(SD)+
SpecTool–Abst(SD)+
58Kuchinke20fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++++
SpecEmotion–Other(LD)++++++
59Kuperberg15fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)+
60Kuperberg9fMRIGenHighM–LowM(SD)+++++
61Laine7PETSpecAnimal–Artifact(SWG)++
62Lee, A10PETSpecPerceptual–Verbal(SWG)++
63Lee, A7PETGenW–N(Mem)+++
64Lee, S12fMRISpecFigurative–Literal(SD)+++
65Liu12fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)
66Luo12fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)+++++
67Majerus12PETGenW–N(Rep)++
68Maril14fMRIGenFam–Unfam(SD)+
69Martin12PETSpecAction–Color(SWG)++++++
70Mashal15fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)+++
SpecFigurative–Unrel(SD)++++
SpecFigurative–Literal(SD)++
71Mechelli20fMRIGenRel–Unrel(Read)++++
72Meyer14fMRIGenW–N(GJ)+
73Miceli9fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)++++
74Mummery10PETGenW(SD)–W(PD)+++++++
SpecLocation–Color(SD)++++
SpecNatural–Artifact(SD)+
75Mummery6PETGenW(SWG)–W(PWG)++
SpecNatural–Artifact(SWG)++
76Nakic13fMRISpecEmotion–Other(LD)++++
77Nichelli9PETGenStory(SD)–Story(OD)+++
78Noppeney23fMRISpecAction–Other(SD)+
SpecVisual–Other(SD)+
79Noppeney27fMRISpecAction–Other(SD)
80Noppeney12PETSpecConc–Abst(SD)
81Noppeney9PETSpecVerbal–Perceptual(SD)++
82Noppeney15fMRISpecAbst–Conc(SD)++
83Obleser16fMRIGenHighM–LowM(Mem)++++
84Orfanidou13fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++++++
85Otten16fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)++
86Paulesu12PETGenW–N(Read)
87Perani9PETGenHighM–LowM(Mem)++
88Perani8PETSpecAnimal–Artifact(Match)++
89Pilgrim15fMRISpecNatural–Artifact(SD)
90Price6PETGenW(SD)–W(PD)+++
91Price6PETGenW(OD)–N(OD)+
92Prince12fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)+++
93Rajah11PETGenW(SD)–W(Rec)+++++
94Raposo15fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)+++++
95Rissman15fMRIGenW–N(LD)++++
96Robertson8fMRISpecStory–UnrelSent(cRead)+
97Rogers12PETSpecSpecific–General(SD)+
98Roskies20PETGenW(SD)–W(PD)+++++++
99Ruschmeyer20fMRISpecAction–Abst(LD)++
100Sabsevitz28fMRISpecConc–Abst(SD)+++++++
101Satpute11fMRISpecCausation–Association(SD)+
102Scott9PETGenW(SD)–W(PD)++++
103Stringaris11fMRIGenHighM–LowM(SD)+++
SpecFigurative–Literal(SD)++++
104Stringaris12fMRISpecFigurative–Literal(SD)++++
105Sugiura16fMRIGenFam–Unfam(SD)++++
SpecSelf–Other(SD)+++
106Thioux6PETSpecAnimal–Number(SD)+++
107Thompson14fMRISpecVerb2–Verb1(LD)+
108Tomasino15fMRISpecAction–Other(SD)+
109Tyler9PETSpecConc–Abst(LD)
110Tyler12fMRISpecAction–Animal(SD)++
SpecAction–Tool(SD)
111Vandenberghe10PETGenLowM–HighM(Match)+
112von Kriegstein14fMRIGenSent(VTarg)–Sent(NVTarg)+
113Wagner13fMRIGenLowA–HighA(SD)+
114Wallentin18fMRISpecConc–Abst(SD)+++++
115Whatmough15PETSpecConc–Abst(SD)++
116Wheatley18fMRISpecAnimal–Artifact(cRead)++++++
117Wise18PETSpecConc–Abst(SD)+
118Woodard15fMRIGenFam–Unfam(SD)+++++++
119Xiao14fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++
120Xu22fMRISpecStory–UnrelSent(Mem)+++++
No.1st AuthorNMethodTypeContrastActivation in brain regions of interest
IPLMTGFG/PHDMPFCIFGVMPFCPC
1Assaf18fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)++++++
2Baumgaertner19fMRISpecAction–Motion(SD)++++
3Baumgaertner9fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++
4Bedny13fMRISpecConc–Abst(SD)++
5Binder30fMRIGenW(SD)–N(PD)++++
6Binder24fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++++++
7Binder24fMRIGenW–N(Read)+++++++
SpecConc–Abst(Read)++++++
8Binder24fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++++++
SpecConc–Abst(LD)+++
9Blaxton14PETGenW(SD)–N(PD)++++++
10Booth13fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)++
11Boronat15fMRISpecAction–Function(SD)+
12Cai15fMRIGenW–N(PD)+
13Cappa13PETGenW(SD)–N(PD)+++
SpecTool–Animal(SD)++
14Cato26fMRISpecEmotion–Neutral(cWG)+++++
15Clark20fMRIGenW–N(PD)+++++
16Collette12PETGenW–N(Rep)+
17Copland14fMRIGenRel–Unrel(LD)+
18Craik8PETGenW(SD)–W(PD)+++++
19Crosson17fMRISpecEmotion–Neutral(cWG)+
20D'Arcy10fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)
21Dapretto8fMRIGenSemSP–SynSP(SD)+
22Daselaar6fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)++++
23Davis12fMRISpecAction–Other(SD)+
24Démonet9PETGenW(SD)–N(PD)++++
25Devlin12fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)+++++
26Devlin26BothSpecArtifact–Natural(SD)
27Ebisch17fMRISpecFunction–Size(SD)++++
28Eschen10fMRISpecAction–Abst(Mem)
29Fiebach12fMRISpecConc–Abst(LD)
30Fiebach11fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++
31Fletcher6PETSpecStory–UnrelSent(SD)++++
SpecThM–Story(SD)+++
32Fliessbach21fMRISpecConc–Abst(Rec)+
33Giesbrecht10fMRISpecConc–Abst(SD)+++
34Giraud12PETGenW–N(Rep)
35Gitelman14fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)++++
36Goldberg13fMRISpecNatural–Other(SD)+++
37Goldberg14fMRISpecColor/Taste/ etc.–Other(SD)++++
38Gorno-Tempini6PETSpecPerson–Object(Match)+
39Gourovitch18PETGenW(SWG)–W(PWG)++++
40Grossman16fMRISpecTool–Animal(SD)+++
41Grossman16fMRISpecMotion–Abst(SD)++
42Hagoort11PETGenW–N(Read)++
43Henke12PETGenW(SD)–W(PD)++
44Henson6fMRIGenW–N(OD)+++
45Henson12fMRIGenW–N(LD)++++
46Herbster10PETGenW–N(cRead)+
47Homae9fMRIGenW/N(LD)–N(Match)+++++
GenSent(SD)–W/N(LD)++++
48Homae10fMRISpecStory(SD)–UnrelSent(LD)+++
49Humphries21fMRIGenHighM–LowM(SD)+++++
GenHighM–N(SD)++++
GenLowM–N(SD)+++
50Ischebeck10fMRIGenW–N(cRead)++++
7GenW–N(LD)++++
51Jessen14fMRISpecConc–Abst(Mem)++++
52Joubert10fMRIGenW–N(cRead)++
53Kellenbach10PETSpecAuditory–Visual(SD)++++
SpecSize–Color(SD)+
54Kelley21fMRISpecSelf–Other(SD)++
55Kosslyn12PETSpecCategory–Other(SD)++
56Kotz13fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++
GenRel–Unrel(LD)+
SpecCategory–Association(LD)++
57Kounios16fMRISpecAnimal–Tool(SD)++
SpecAnimal–Abst(SD)+
SpecTool–Abst(SD)+
58Kuchinke20fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++++
SpecEmotion–Other(LD)++++++
59Kuperberg15fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)+
60Kuperberg9fMRIGenHighM–LowM(SD)+++++
61Laine7PETSpecAnimal–Artifact(SWG)++
62Lee, A10PETSpecPerceptual–Verbal(SWG)++
63Lee, A7PETGenW–N(Mem)+++
64Lee, S12fMRISpecFigurative–Literal(SD)+++
65Liu12fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)
66Luo12fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)+++++
67Majerus12PETGenW–N(Rep)++
68Maril14fMRIGenFam–Unfam(SD)+
69Martin12PETSpecAction–Color(SWG)++++++
70Mashal15fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)+++
SpecFigurative–Unrel(SD)++++
SpecFigurative–Literal(SD)++
71Mechelli20fMRIGenRel–Unrel(Read)++++
72Meyer14fMRIGenW–N(GJ)+
73Miceli9fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)++++
74Mummery10PETGenW(SD)–W(PD)+++++++
SpecLocation–Color(SD)++++
SpecNatural–Artifact(SD)+
75Mummery6PETGenW(SWG)–W(PWG)++
SpecNatural–Artifact(SWG)++
76Nakic13fMRISpecEmotion–Other(LD)++++
77Nichelli9PETGenStory(SD)–Story(OD)+++
78Noppeney23fMRISpecAction–Other(SD)+
SpecVisual–Other(SD)+
79Noppeney27fMRISpecAction–Other(SD)
80Noppeney12PETSpecConc–Abst(SD)
81Noppeney9PETSpecVerbal–Perceptual(SD)++
82Noppeney15fMRISpecAbst–Conc(SD)++
83Obleser16fMRIGenHighM–LowM(Mem)++++
84Orfanidou13fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++++++
85Otten16fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)++
86Paulesu12PETGenW–N(Read)
87Perani9PETGenHighM–LowM(Mem)++
88Perani8PETSpecAnimal–Artifact(Match)++
89Pilgrim15fMRISpecNatural–Artifact(SD)
90Price6PETGenW(SD)–W(PD)+++
91Price6PETGenW(OD)–N(OD)+
92Prince12fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)+++
93Rajah11PETGenW(SD)–W(Rec)+++++
94Raposo15fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)+++++
95Rissman15fMRIGenW–N(LD)++++
96Robertson8fMRISpecStory–UnrelSent(cRead)+
97Rogers12PETSpecSpecific–General(SD)+
98Roskies20PETGenW(SD)–W(PD)+++++++
99Ruschmeyer20fMRISpecAction–Abst(LD)++
100Sabsevitz28fMRISpecConc–Abst(SD)+++++++
101Satpute11fMRISpecCausation–Association(SD)+
102Scott9PETGenW(SD)–W(PD)++++
103Stringaris11fMRIGenHighM–LowM(SD)+++
SpecFigurative–Literal(SD)++++
104Stringaris12fMRISpecFigurative–Literal(SD)++++
105Sugiura16fMRIGenFam–Unfam(SD)++++
SpecSelf–Other(SD)+++
106Thioux6PETSpecAnimal–Number(SD)+++
107Thompson14fMRISpecVerb2–Verb1(LD)+
108Tomasino15fMRISpecAction–Other(SD)+
109Tyler9PETSpecConc–Abst(LD)
110Tyler12fMRISpecAction–Animal(SD)++
SpecAction–Tool(SD)
111Vandenberghe10PETGenLowM–HighM(Match)+
112von Kriegstein14fMRIGenSent(VTarg)–Sent(NVTarg)+
113Wagner13fMRIGenLowA–HighA(SD)+
114Wallentin18fMRISpecConc–Abst(SD)+++++
115Whatmough15PETSpecConc–Abst(SD)++
116Wheatley18fMRISpecAnimal–Artifact(cRead)++++++
117Wise18PETSpecConc–Abst(SD)+
118Woodard15fMRIGenFam–Unfam(SD)+++++++
119Xiao14fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++
120Xu22fMRISpecStory–UnrelSent(Mem)+++++

Note: The Contrast column provides a code indicating the contrast used, with tasks indicated in parentheses after stimuli. The terms Action, Animal, Artifact, Association, Auditory, Category, Causation, Color, Emotion, Figurative, Function, General, Literal, Location, Motion, Natural, Number, Perceptual, Self, Size, Specific, Taste, Tool, Verbal, and Visual refer to the object category or knowledge domain emphasized by the stimulus or task. “Other” indicates a miscellaneous combination of categories or domains. Abbreviations used for stimuli: Abst = abstract words, Conc = concrete words, Fam = familiar concepts, Unfam = unfamiliar concepts, N = nonwords (i.e., pseudowords), W = words, HighM = sentences or words with high meaningfulness, LowM = sentences or words with low meaningfulness, HighA = highly associated word pairs, LowA = weakly associated word pairs, Rel = semantically related words, Unrel = semantically unrelated words, Sent = sentences, Story = connected narrative or discourse, UnrelSent = unrelated sentences, SemSP = semantically distinct sentence pairs, SynSP = syntactically distinct sentence pairs, ThM = theory-of-mind narrative, Verb1 = single-argument verb, Verb2 = 2-argument verb. Abbreviations for tasks: GJ = grammaticality judgment, LD = lexical decision, Match = identity matching, Mem = memorization, OD = orthographic decision, PD = phonological decision, cRead = covert reading, Read = oral reading, Rep = oral repetition, Rec = recognition, SD = semantic decision, PWG = phonological word generation, SWG = semantic word generation, cSWG = covert semantic word generation, VTarg = detect a target word or phrase, NVTarg = detect a target nonverbal feature. Examples: W(SD)–N(PD) indicates a semantic decision task on words contrasted with a phonological decision on pseudowords; Conc–Abst(LD) indicates a contrast between concrete and abstract words presented during a lexical decision task. Specific contrasts were almost always performed in both directions (e.g., Animal–Tool and Tool–Animal); for simplicity such complementary contrasts are collapsed into a single row in the table.

Other abbreviations: N = number of subjects in study; Gen = general contrast; Spec = specific contrast; IPL = inferior parietal lobe (angular and supramarginal gyri); MTG = middle temporal gyrus; FG/PH = fusiform and parahippocampal gyri; DMPFC = dorsomedial prefrontal cortex; IFG = inferior frontal gyrus; VMPFC = ventromedial prefrontal cortex; PC = posterior cingulate gyrus.

Appendix 2. Individual Study Details
No.1st AuthorNMethodTypeContrastActivation in brain regions of interest
IPLMTGFG/PHDMPFCIFGVMPFCPC
1Assaf18fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)++++++
2Baumgaertner19fMRISpecAction–Motion(SD)++++
3Baumgaertner9fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++
4Bedny13fMRISpecConc–Abst(SD)++
5Binder30fMRIGenW(SD)–N(PD)++++
6Binder24fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++++++
7Binder24fMRIGenW–N(Read)+++++++
SpecConc–Abst(Read)++++++
8Binder24fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++++++
SpecConc–Abst(LD)+++
9Blaxton14PETGenW(SD)–N(PD)++++++
10Booth13fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)++
11Boronat15fMRISpecAction–Function(SD)+
12Cai15fMRIGenW–N(PD)+
13Cappa13PETGenW(SD)–N(PD)+++
SpecTool–Animal(SD)++
14Cato26fMRISpecEmotion–Neutral(cWG)+++++
15Clark20fMRIGenW–N(PD)+++++
16Collette12PETGenW–N(Rep)+
17Copland14fMRIGenRel–Unrel(LD)+
18Craik8PETGenW(SD)–W(PD)+++++
19Crosson17fMRISpecEmotion–Neutral(cWG)+
20D'Arcy10fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)
21Dapretto8fMRIGenSemSP–SynSP(SD)+
22Daselaar6fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)++++
23Davis12fMRISpecAction–Other(SD)+
24Démonet9PETGenW(SD)–N(PD)++++
25Devlin12fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)+++++
26Devlin26BothSpecArtifact–Natural(SD)
27Ebisch17fMRISpecFunction–Size(SD)++++
28Eschen10fMRISpecAction–Abst(Mem)
29Fiebach12fMRISpecConc–Abst(LD)
30Fiebach11fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++
31Fletcher6PETSpecStory–UnrelSent(SD)++++
SpecThM–Story(SD)+++
32Fliessbach21fMRISpecConc–Abst(Rec)+
33Giesbrecht10fMRISpecConc–Abst(SD)+++
34Giraud12PETGenW–N(Rep)
35Gitelman14fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)++++
36Goldberg13fMRISpecNatural–Other(SD)+++
37Goldberg14fMRISpecColor/Taste/ etc.–Other(SD)++++
38Gorno-Tempini6PETSpecPerson–Object(Match)+
39Gourovitch18PETGenW(SWG)–W(PWG)++++
40Grossman16fMRISpecTool–Animal(SD)+++
41Grossman16fMRISpecMotion–Abst(SD)++
42Hagoort11PETGenW–N(Read)++
43Henke12PETGenW(SD)–W(PD)++
44Henson6fMRIGenW–N(OD)+++
45Henson12fMRIGenW–N(LD)++++
46Herbster10PETGenW–N(cRead)+
47Homae9fMRIGenW/N(LD)–N(Match)+++++
GenSent(SD)–W/N(LD)++++
48Homae10fMRISpecStory(SD)–UnrelSent(LD)+++
49Humphries21fMRIGenHighM–LowM(SD)+++++
GenHighM–N(SD)++++
GenLowM–N(SD)+++
50Ischebeck10fMRIGenW–N(cRead)++++
7GenW–N(LD)++++
51Jessen14fMRISpecConc–Abst(Mem)++++
52Joubert10fMRIGenW–N(cRead)++
53Kellenbach10PETSpecAuditory–Visual(SD)++++
SpecSize–Color(SD)+
54Kelley21fMRISpecSelf–Other(SD)++
55Kosslyn12PETSpecCategory–Other(SD)++
56Kotz13fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++
GenRel–Unrel(LD)+
SpecCategory–Association(LD)++
57Kounios16fMRISpecAnimal–Tool(SD)++
SpecAnimal–Abst(SD)+
SpecTool–Abst(SD)+
58Kuchinke20fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++++
SpecEmotion–Other(LD)++++++
59Kuperberg15fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)+
60Kuperberg9fMRIGenHighM–LowM(SD)+++++
61Laine7PETSpecAnimal–Artifact(SWG)++
62Lee, A10PETSpecPerceptual–Verbal(SWG)++
63Lee, A7PETGenW–N(Mem)+++
64Lee, S12fMRISpecFigurative–Literal(SD)+++
65Liu12fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)
66Luo12fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)+++++
67Majerus12PETGenW–N(Rep)++
68Maril14fMRIGenFam–Unfam(SD)+
69Martin12PETSpecAction–Color(SWG)++++++
70Mashal15fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)+++
SpecFigurative–Unrel(SD)++++
SpecFigurative–Literal(SD)++
71Mechelli20fMRIGenRel–Unrel(Read)++++
72Meyer14fMRIGenW–N(GJ)+
73Miceli9fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)++++
74Mummery10PETGenW(SD)–W(PD)+++++++
SpecLocation–Color(SD)++++
SpecNatural–Artifact(SD)+
75Mummery6PETGenW(SWG)–W(PWG)++
SpecNatural–Artifact(SWG)++
76Nakic13fMRISpecEmotion–Other(LD)++++
77Nichelli9PETGenStory(SD)–Story(OD)+++
78Noppeney23fMRISpecAction–Other(SD)+
SpecVisual–Other(SD)+
79Noppeney27fMRISpecAction–Other(SD)
80Noppeney12PETSpecConc–Abst(SD)
81Noppeney9PETSpecVerbal–Perceptual(SD)++
82Noppeney15fMRISpecAbst–Conc(SD)++
83Obleser16fMRIGenHighM–LowM(Mem)++++
84Orfanidou13fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++++++
85Otten16fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)++
86Paulesu12PETGenW–N(Read)
87Perani9PETGenHighM–LowM(Mem)++
88Perani8PETSpecAnimal–Artifact(Match)++
89Pilgrim15fMRISpecNatural–Artifact(SD)
90Price6PETGenW(SD)–W(PD)+++
91Price6PETGenW(OD)–N(OD)+
92Prince12fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)+++
93Rajah11PETGenW(SD)–W(Rec)+++++
94Raposo15fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)+++++
95Rissman15fMRIGenW–N(LD)++++
96Robertson8fMRISpecStory–UnrelSent(cRead)+
97Rogers12PETSpecSpecific–General(SD)+
98Roskies20PETGenW(SD)–W(PD)+++++++
99Ruschmeyer20fMRISpecAction–Abst(LD)++
100Sabsevitz28fMRISpecConc–Abst(SD)+++++++
101Satpute11fMRISpecCausation–Association(SD)+
102Scott9PETGenW(SD)–W(PD)++++
103Stringaris11fMRIGenHighM–LowM(SD)+++
SpecFigurative–Literal(SD)++++
104Stringaris12fMRISpecFigurative–Literal(SD)++++
105Sugiura16fMRIGenFam–Unfam(SD)++++
SpecSelf–Other(SD)+++
106Thioux6PETSpecAnimal–Number(SD)+++
107Thompson14fMRISpecVerb2–Verb1(LD)+
108Tomasino15fMRISpecAction–Other(SD)+
109Tyler9PETSpecConc–Abst(LD)
110Tyler12fMRISpecAction–Animal(SD)++
SpecAction–Tool(SD)
111Vandenberghe10PETGenLowM–HighM(Match)+
112von Kriegstein14fMRIGenSent(VTarg)–Sent(NVTarg)+
113Wagner13fMRIGenLowA–HighA(SD)+
114Wallentin18fMRISpecConc–Abst(SD)+++++
115Whatmough15PETSpecConc–Abst(SD)++
116Wheatley18fMRISpecAnimal–Artifact(cRead)++++++
117Wise18PETSpecConc–Abst(SD)+
118Woodard15fMRIGenFam–Unfam(SD)+++++++
119Xiao14fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++
120Xu22fMRISpecStory–UnrelSent(Mem)+++++
No.1st AuthorNMethodTypeContrastActivation in brain regions of interest
IPLMTGFG/PHDMPFCIFGVMPFCPC
1Assaf18fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)++++++
2Baumgaertner19fMRISpecAction–Motion(SD)++++
3Baumgaertner9fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++
4Bedny13fMRISpecConc–Abst(SD)++
5Binder30fMRIGenW(SD)–N(PD)++++
6Binder24fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++++++
7Binder24fMRIGenW–N(Read)+++++++
SpecConc–Abst(Read)++++++
8Binder24fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++++++
SpecConc–Abst(LD)+++
9Blaxton14PETGenW(SD)–N(PD)++++++
10Booth13fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)++
11Boronat15fMRISpecAction–Function(SD)+
12Cai15fMRIGenW–N(PD)+
13Cappa13PETGenW(SD)–N(PD)+++
SpecTool–Animal(SD)++
14Cato26fMRISpecEmotion–Neutral(cWG)+++++
15Clark20fMRIGenW–N(PD)+++++
16Collette12PETGenW–N(Rep)+
17Copland14fMRIGenRel–Unrel(LD)+
18Craik8PETGenW(SD)–W(PD)+++++
19Crosson17fMRISpecEmotion–Neutral(cWG)+
20D'Arcy10fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)
21Dapretto8fMRIGenSemSP–SynSP(SD)+
22Daselaar6fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)++++
23Davis12fMRISpecAction–Other(SD)+
24Démonet9PETGenW(SD)–N(PD)++++
25Devlin12fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)+++++
26Devlin26BothSpecArtifact–Natural(SD)
27Ebisch17fMRISpecFunction–Size(SD)++++
28Eschen10fMRISpecAction–Abst(Mem)
29Fiebach12fMRISpecConc–Abst(LD)
30Fiebach11fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++
31Fletcher6PETSpecStory–UnrelSent(SD)++++
SpecThM–Story(SD)+++
32Fliessbach21fMRISpecConc–Abst(Rec)+
33Giesbrecht10fMRISpecConc–Abst(SD)+++
34Giraud12PETGenW–N(Rep)
35Gitelman14fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)++++
36Goldberg13fMRISpecNatural–Other(SD)+++
37Goldberg14fMRISpecColor/Taste/ etc.–Other(SD)++++
38Gorno-Tempini6PETSpecPerson–Object(Match)+
39Gourovitch18PETGenW(SWG)–W(PWG)++++
40Grossman16fMRISpecTool–Animal(SD)+++
41Grossman16fMRISpecMotion–Abst(SD)++
42Hagoort11PETGenW–N(Read)++
43Henke12PETGenW(SD)–W(PD)++
44Henson6fMRIGenW–N(OD)+++
45Henson12fMRIGenW–N(LD)++++
46Herbster10PETGenW–N(cRead)+
47Homae9fMRIGenW/N(LD)–N(Match)+++++
GenSent(SD)–W/N(LD)++++
48Homae10fMRISpecStory(SD)–UnrelSent(LD)+++
49Humphries21fMRIGenHighM–LowM(SD)+++++
GenHighM–N(SD)++++
GenLowM–N(SD)+++
50Ischebeck10fMRIGenW–N(cRead)++++
7GenW–N(LD)++++
51Jessen14fMRISpecConc–Abst(Mem)++++
52Joubert10fMRIGenW–N(cRead)++
53Kellenbach10PETSpecAuditory–Visual(SD)++++
SpecSize–Color(SD)+
54Kelley21fMRISpecSelf–Other(SD)++
55Kosslyn12PETSpecCategory–Other(SD)++
56Kotz13fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++
GenRel–Unrel(LD)+
SpecCategory–Association(LD)++
57Kounios16fMRISpecAnimal–Tool(SD)++
SpecAnimal–Abst(SD)+
SpecTool–Abst(SD)+
58Kuchinke20fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++++
SpecEmotion–Other(LD)++++++
59Kuperberg15fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)+
60Kuperberg9fMRIGenHighM–LowM(SD)+++++
61Laine7PETSpecAnimal–Artifact(SWG)++
62Lee, A10PETSpecPerceptual–Verbal(SWG)++
63Lee, A7PETGenW–N(Mem)+++
64Lee, S12fMRISpecFigurative–Literal(SD)+++
65Liu12fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)
66Luo12fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)+++++
67Majerus12PETGenW–N(Rep)++
68Maril14fMRIGenFam–Unfam(SD)+
69Martin12PETSpecAction–Color(SWG)++++++
70Mashal15fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)+++
SpecFigurative–Unrel(SD)++++
SpecFigurative–Literal(SD)++
71Mechelli20fMRIGenRel–Unrel(Read)++++
72Meyer14fMRIGenW–N(GJ)+
73Miceli9fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)++++
74Mummery10PETGenW(SD)–W(PD)+++++++
SpecLocation–Color(SD)++++
SpecNatural–Artifact(SD)+
75Mummery6PETGenW(SWG)–W(PWG)++
SpecNatural–Artifact(SWG)++
76Nakic13fMRISpecEmotion–Other(LD)++++
77Nichelli9PETGenStory(SD)–Story(OD)+++
78Noppeney23fMRISpecAction–Other(SD)+
SpecVisual–Other(SD)+
79Noppeney27fMRISpecAction–Other(SD)
80Noppeney12PETSpecConc–Abst(SD)
81Noppeney9PETSpecVerbal–Perceptual(SD)++
82Noppeney15fMRISpecAbst–Conc(SD)++
83Obleser16fMRIGenHighM–LowM(Mem)++++
84Orfanidou13fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++++++
85Otten16fMRIGenW(SD)–W(PD)++
86Paulesu12PETGenW–N(Read)
87Perani9PETGenHighM–LowM(Mem)++
88Perani8PETSpecAnimal–Artifact(Match)++
89Pilgrim15fMRISpecNatural–Artifact(SD)
90Price6PETGenW(SD)–W(PD)+++
91Price6PETGenW(OD)–N(OD)+
92Prince12fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)+++
93Rajah11PETGenW(SD)–W(Rec)+++++
94Raposo15fMRIGenRel–Unrel(SD)+++++
95Rissman15fMRIGenW–N(LD)++++
96Robertson8fMRISpecStory–UnrelSent(cRead)+
97Rogers12PETSpecSpecific–General(SD)+
98Roskies20PETGenW(SD)–W(PD)+++++++
99Ruschmeyer20fMRISpecAction–Abst(LD)++
100Sabsevitz28fMRISpecConc–Abst(SD)+++++++
101Satpute11fMRISpecCausation–Association(SD)+
102Scott9PETGenW(SD)–W(PD)++++
103Stringaris11fMRIGenHighM–LowM(SD)+++
SpecFigurative–Literal(SD)++++
104Stringaris12fMRISpecFigurative–Literal(SD)++++
105Sugiura16fMRIGenFam–Unfam(SD)++++
SpecSelf–Other(SD)+++
106Thioux6PETSpecAnimal–Number(SD)+++
107Thompson14fMRISpecVerb2–Verb1(LD)+
108Tomasino15fMRISpecAction–Other(SD)+
109Tyler9PETSpecConc–Abst(LD)
110Tyler12fMRISpecAction–Animal(SD)++
SpecAction–Tool(SD)
111Vandenberghe10PETGenLowM–HighM(Match)+
112von Kriegstein14fMRIGenSent(VTarg)–Sent(NVTarg)+
113Wagner13fMRIGenLowA–HighA(SD)+
114Wallentin18fMRISpecConc–Abst(SD)+++++
115Whatmough15PETSpecConc–Abst(SD)++
116Wheatley18fMRISpecAnimal–Artifact(cRead)++++++
117Wise18PETSpecConc–Abst(SD)+
118Woodard15fMRIGenFam–Unfam(SD)+++++++
119Xiao14fMRIGenW–N(LD)+++
120Xu22fMRISpecStory–UnrelSent(Mem)+++++

Note: The Contrast column provides a code indicating the contrast used, with tasks indicated in parentheses after stimuli. The terms Action, Animal, Artifact, Association, Auditory, Category, Causation, Color, Emotion, Figurative, Function, General, Literal, Location, Motion, Natural, Number, Perceptual, Self, Size, Specific, Taste, Tool, Verbal, and Visual refer to the object category or knowledge domain emphasized by the stimulus or task. “Other” indicates a miscellaneous combination of categories or domains. Abbreviations used for stimuli: Abst = abstract words, Conc = concrete words, Fam = familiar concepts, Unfam = unfamiliar concepts, N = nonwords (i.e., pseudowords), W = words, HighM = sentences or words with high meaningfulness, LowM = sentences or words with low meaningfulness, HighA = highly associated word pairs, LowA = weakly associated word pairs, Rel = semantically related words, Unrel = semantically unrelated words, Sent = sentences, Story = connected narrative or discourse, UnrelSent = unrelated sentences, SemSP = semantically distinct sentence pairs, SynSP = syntactically distinct sentence pairs, ThM = theory-of-mind narrative, Verb1 = single-argument verb, Verb2 = 2-argument verb. Abbreviations for tasks: GJ = grammaticality judgment, LD = lexical decision, Match = identity matching, Mem = memorization, OD = orthographic decision, PD = phonological decision, cRead = covert reading, Read = oral reading, Rep = oral repetition, Rec = recognition, SD = semantic decision, PWG = phonological word generation, SWG = semantic word generation, cSWG = covert semantic word generation, VTarg = detect a target word or phrase, NVTarg = detect a target nonverbal feature. Examples: W(SD)–N(PD) indicates a semantic decision task on words contrasted with a phonological decision on pseudowords; Conc–Abst(LD) indicates a contrast between concrete and abstract words presented during a lexical decision task. Specific contrasts were almost always performed in both directions (e.g., Animal–Tool and Tool–Animal); for simplicity such complementary contrasts are collapsed into a single row in the table.

Other abbreviations: N = number of subjects in study; Gen = general contrast; Spec = specific contrast; IPL = inferior parietal lobe (angular and supramarginal gyri); MTG = middle temporal gyrus; FG/PH = fusiform and parahippocampal gyri; DMPFC = dorsomedial prefrontal cortex; IFG = inferior frontal gyrus; VMPFC = ventromedial prefrontal cortex; PC = posterior cingulate gyrus.

References

Addis
DR
Moscovitch
M
Crawley
AP
McAndrews
MP
Recollective qualities modulate hippocampal activation during autobiographical memory retrieval
Hippocampus
2004
, vol. 
14
 (pg. 
752
-
762
)
Adler
CM
Sax
KW
Holland
SK
Schmithorst
V
Rosenberg
L
Strakowski
SM
Changes in neuronal activation with increasing attention demand in healthy volunteers: an fMRI study
Synapse
2001
, vol. 
42
 (pg. 
266
-
272
)
Aggleton
JP
Pearce
JM
Neural systems underlying episodic memory: insights from animal research
Phil Trans Roy Soc Lond B
2001
, vol. 
356
 (pg. 
1467
-
1482
)
Alexander
MP
Benson
DF
Joynt
RJ
The aphasias and related disturbances
Clinical Neurology
1993
Philadelphia (PA)
J.B. Lipincott
(pg. 
1
-
58
)
Alexander
MP
Hiltbrunner
B
Fischer
RS
Distributed anatomy of transcortical sensory aphasia
Arch Neurol
1989
, vol. 
46
 (pg. 
885
-
892
)
Andersen
RA
Asanuma
C
Essick
G
Siegel
RM
Corticocortical connections of anatomically and physiologically defined subdivisions within the inferior parietal lobule
J Comp Neurol
1990
, vol. 
296
 (pg. 
65
-
113
)
Andersen
RA
Snyder
LH
Bradley
DC
Xing
J
Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements
Annual Rev Neurosci
1997
, vol. 
20
 (pg. 
303
-
330
)
Andreasen
NC
O'Leary
DS
Cizadlo
T
Arndt
S
Rezai
K
Watkins
GL
Boles Ponto
LL
Hichwa
RD
Remembering the past: two facets of episodic memory explored with positron emission tomography
Am J Psychiatry
1995
, vol. 
152
 (pg. 
1576
-
1585
)
Antrobus
JS
Information theory and stimulus-independent thought
Br J Psychol
1968
, vol. 
59
 (pg. 
423
-
430
)
Antrobus
JS
Singer
JL
Greenberg
S
Studies in the stream of consciousness: experimental enhancement and suppression of spontaneous cognitive processes
Perceptual Motor Skills
1966
, vol. 
23
 (pg. 
399
-
417
)
Attwell
D
Laughlin
SB
An energy budget for signaling in the grey matter of the brain
J Cereb Blood Flow Metab
2001
, vol. 
21
 (pg. 
1133
-
1145
)
Aurell
CG
Perception: a model comprising two modes of consciousness
Percept Motor Skills
1979
, vol. 
49
 (pg. 
431
-
444
)
Baleydier
C
Mauguiere
F
The duality of the cingulate gyrus in the monkey. Neuroanatomical study and functional hypothesis
Brain
1980
, vol. 
103
 (pg. 
525
-
554
)
Barbas
H
Organization of cortical afferent input to orbitofrontal areas in the rhesus monkey
Neuroscience
1993
, vol. 
56
 (pg. 
841
-
864
)
Barch
DM
Braver
TS
Akbudak
E
Conturo
T
Ollinger
J
Snyder
A
Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of response modality and processing domain
Cereb Cortex
2001
, vol. 
11
 (pg. 
837
-
848
)
Barsalou
LW
Neisser
U
Winograd
E
The content and organization of autobiographical memories
Remembering reconsidered: ecological and traditional approaches to the study of memory
1998
Cambridge
Cambridge University Press
(pg. 
193
-
243
)
Baumgaertner
A
Buccino
G
Lange
R
McNamara
A
Binkofski
F
Polymodal conceptual processing of human biological actions in the left inferior frontal lobe
Eur J Neurosci
2007
, vol. 
25
 (pg. 
881
-
889
)
Baumgaertner
A
Weiller
C
Buchel
C
Event-related fMRI reveals cortical sites involved in contextual sentence integration
Neuroimage
2002
, vol. 
16
 (pg. 
736
-
745
)
Baylis
GC
Rolls
ET
Leonard
CM
Functional subdivisions of the temporal lobe neocortex
J Neurosci
1987
, vol. 
7
 (pg. 
330
-
342
)
Bechara
A
Damasio
H
Damasio
AR
Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex
Cereb Cortex
2000
, vol. 
10
 (pg. 
295
-
307
)
Bedny
M
Thompson-Schill
SL
Neuroanatomically separable effects of imageability and grammatical class during single-word comprehension
Brain Lang
2006
, vol. 
98
 (pg. 
127
-
139
)
Benson
DF
Aphasia, alexia and agraphia
1979
New York
Churchill Livingstone
Benson
DF
Cummings
JL
Tsai
SY
Angular gyrus syndrome simulating Alzheimer's disease
Arch Neurol
1982
, vol. 
38
 (pg. 
616
-
620
)
Benton
AL
The fiction of the “Gerstmann syndrome”
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
1961
, vol. 
24
 (pg. 
176
-
181
)
Berthier
ML
Transcortical aphasias
1999
Hove
Psychology Press
Binder
JR
D'Esposito
ME
Wernicke aphasia: a disorder of central language processing
Neurological foundations of cognitive neuroscience
2002
Cambridge (MA)
MIT Press
(pg. 
175
-
238
)
Binder
JR
Hart
J
Kraut
MA
Effects of word imageability on semantic access: neuroimaging studies
Neural basis of semantic memory
2007
Cambridge, UK
Cambridge University Press
(pg. 
149
-
181
)
Binder
JR
Frost
JA
Hammeke
TA
Bellgowan
PSF
Rao
SM
Cox
RW
Conceptual processing during the conscious resting state: a functional MRI study
J Cogn Neurosci
1999
, vol. 
11
 (pg. 
80
-
93
)
Binder
JR
Frost
JA
Hammeke
TA
Bellgowan
PSF
Springer
JA
Kaufman
JN
Possing
ET
Human temporal lobe activation by speech and nonspeech sounds
Cereb Cortex
2000
, vol. 
10
 (pg. 
512
-
528
)
Binder
JR
Frost
JA
Hammeke
TA
Cox
RW
Rao
SM
Prieto
T
Human brain language areas identified by functional MRI
J Neurosci
1997
, vol. 
17
 (pg. 
353
-
362
)
Binder
JR
Liebenthal
E
Possing
ET
Medler
DA
Ward
BD
Neural correlates of sensory and decision processes in auditory object identification
Nature Neurosci
2004
, vol. 
7
 (pg. 
295
-
301
)
Binder
JR
Medler
DA
Desai
R
Conant
LL
Liebenthal
E
Some neurophysiological constraints on models of word naming
Neuroimage
2005
, vol. 
27
 (pg. 
677
-
693
)
Binder
JR
Swanson
SJ
Hammeke
TA
Sabsevitz
DS
A comparison of five fMRI protocols for mapping speech comprehension systems
Epilepsia
2008
, vol. 
49
 (pg. 
1980
-
1997
)
Binder
JR
Westbury
CF
Possing
ET
McKiernan
KA
Medler
DA
Distinct brain systems for processing concrete and abstract concepts
J Cogn Neurosci
2005
, vol. 
17
 (pg. 
905
-
917
)
Binkofski
F
Buccino
G
Posse
S
Seitz
RJ
Rizzolatti
G
Freund
HJ
A fronto-parietal circuit for object manipulation in man: evidence from an fMRI study
Eur J Neurosci
1999
, vol. 
11
 (pg. 
3276
-
3286
)
Blatt
GJ
Pandya
DN
Rosene
DL
Parcellation of cortical afferents to three distinct sectors in the parahippocampal gyrus of the rhesus monkey: an anatomical and neurophysiological study
J Comp Neurol
2003
, vol. 
466
 (pg. 
161
-
179
)
Bock
M
The influence of emotional meaning on the recall of words processed for form or self-reference
Psychol Res
1986
, vol. 
48
 (pg. 
107
-
112
)
Bogen
JE
Bogen
GM
Wernicke's region—where is it?
Ann NY Acad Sci
1976
, vol. 
290
 (pg. 
834
-
843
)
Bookheimer
SY
Functional MRI of language: new approaches to understanding the cortical organization of semantic processing
Annu Rev Neurosci
2002
, vol. 
25
 (pg. 
151
-
188
)
Boronat
CB
Buxbaum
LJ
Coslett
HB
Tang
K
Saffran
EM
Kimberg
DY
Detre
JA
Distinctions between manipulation and function knowledge of objects: evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging
Cogn Brain Res
2005
, vol. 
23
 (pg. 
361
-
373
)
Braver
TS
Barch
DM
Gray
JR
Molfese
DL
Snyder
A
Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of frequency, inhibition and errors
Cereb Cortex
2001
, vol. 
11
 (pg. 
825
-
836
)
Braver
TS
Cohen
JD
Nystrom
LE
Jonides
J
Smith
EE
Noll
DC
A parametric study of prefrontal cortex involvement in human working memory
Neuroimage
1997
, vol. 
5
 (pg. 
49
-
62
)
Bréal
M
Essai de sémantique (science des significations)
1897
Paris
Librairie Hachette
Breedin
SD
Saffran
EM
Coslett
HB
Reversal of a concreteness effect in a patient with semantic dementia
Cogn Neuropsychol
1995
, vol. 
11
 (pg. 
617
-
660
)
Bright
P
Moss
HE
Tyler
LK
Unitary vs. multiple semantics: PET studies of word and picture processing
Brain Lang
2004
, vol. 
89
 (pg. 
417
-
432
)
Broca
P
Remarques sur le siège de la faculté du langage articulé; suivies d'une observation d'aphemie
Bull Soc Anat Paris
1861
, vol. 
6
 (pg. 
330
-
357
)
Brodmann
K
Localisation in the cerebral cortex
1994
London
Smith-Gordon
Buchsbaum
BR
D'Esposito
M
The search for the phonological store: from loop to convolution
J Cogn Neurosci
2008
, vol. 
20
 (pg. 
762
-
778
)
Buckner
RL
Raichle
ME
Petersen
SE
Dissociation of human prefrontal cortical areas across different speech production tasks and gender groups
J Neurosci
1995
, vol. 
74
 (pg. 
2163
-
2173
)
Burgess
N
Spatial cognition and the brain
Ann NY Acad Sci
2008
, vol. 
1124
 (pg. 
77
-
97
)
Burton
MW
Small
S
Blumstein
SE
The role of segmentation in phonological processing: an fMRI investigation
J Cogn Neurosci
2000
, vol. 
12
 (pg. 
679
-
690
)
Buxbaum
LJ
Ideomotor apraxia: a call to action
Neurocase
2001
, vol. 
7
 (pg. 
445
-
458
)
Buxbaum
LJ
Kyle
KM
Menon
R
On beyond mirror neurons: internal representations subserving imitation and recognition of skilled object-related actions in humans
Cogn Brain Res
2005
, vol. 
25
 (pg. 
226
-
239
)
Buxbaum
LJ
Kyle
KM
Tang
K
Detre
JA
Neural substrates of knowledge of hand postures for object grasping and functional onject use: evidence from fMRI
Brain Res
2006
, vol. 
1117
 (pg. 
175
-
185
)
Cabeza
R
Nyberg
L
Imaging cognition II: an empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies
J Cogn Neurosci
2000
, vol. 
12
 (pg. 
1
-
47
)
Cappa
SF
Perani
D
Schnur
T
Tettamanti
M
Fazio
F
The effects of semantic category and knowledge type on lexical–semantic access: a PET study
Neuroimage
1998
, vol. 
8
 (pg. 
350
-
359
)
Cappe
C
Barone
P
Heteromodal connections supporting multisensory integration at low levels of cortical processing in the monkey
Eur J Neurosci
2005
, vol. 
22
 (pg. 
2886
-
2902
)
Caramazza
A
Berndt
RS
Basili
AG
Koller
JJ
Syntactic processing deficits in aphasia
Cortex
1981
, vol. 
17
 (pg. 
333
-
348
)
Carter
CS
Botvinick
MM
Cohen
JD
The contribution of the anterior cingulate cortex to executive processes in cognition
Rev Neurosci
1999
, vol. 
10
 (pg. 
49
-
57
)
Caspers
S
Geyer
S
Schleicher
A
Mohlberg
H
Amunts
K
Zilles
K
The human inferior parietal cortex: cytoarchitectonic parcellation and interindividual variability
Neuroimage
2006
, vol. 
33
 (pg. 
430
-
448
)
Cavada
C
The anatomical connections of the macaque monkey orbitofrontal cortex. A review
Cereb Cortex
2000
, vol. 
10
 (pg. 
220
-
242
)
Cavada
C
Goldman-Rakic
PS
Posterior parietal cortex in rhesus monkey: i. Parcellation of areas based on distinctive limbic and sensory corticocortical connections
J Comp Neurol
1989
, vol. 
287
 (pg. 
393
-
421
)
Cavada
C
Goldman-Rakic
PS
Posterior parietal cortex in the rhesus monkey: iI. Evidence for segregated corticocortical networks linking sensory and limbic areas with the frontal lobe
J Comp Neurol
1989
, vol. 
287
 (pg. 
422
-
445
)
Chafee
M
Goldman-Rakic
PS
Matching patterns of activity in primate prefrontal area 8a and parietal area 7ip neurons during a spatial working memory task
J Neurophysiol
1998
, vol. 
79
 (pg. 
2919
-
2940
)
Chance
FS
Abbott
LF
Reyes
AD
Gain modulation from background synaptic input
Neuron
2002
, vol. 
35
 (pg. 
773
-
782
)
Chao
LL
Haxby
JV
Martin
A
Attribute-based neural substrates in temporal cortex for perceiving and knowing about objects
Nature Neurosci
1999
, vol. 
2
 (pg. 
913
-
919
)
Chao
LL
Martin
A
Cortical regions associated with perceiving, naming, and knowing about colors
J Cogn Neurosci
1999
, vol. 
11
 (pg. 
25
-
35
)
Chao
LL
Martin
A
Representation of manipulable man-made objects in the dorsal stream
Neuroimage
2000
, vol. 
12
 (pg. 
478
-
484
)
Chao
LL
Weisberg
J
Martin
A
Experience-dependent modulation of category-related cortical activity
Cereb Cortex
2002
, vol. 
12
 (pg. 
545
-
551
)
Chee
MW
Hon
NHH
Caplan
D
Lee
HL
Goh
J
Frequency of concrete words modulates prefrontal activation during semantic judgments
Neuroimage
2002
, vol. 
16
 (pg. 
259
-
268
)
Chee
MW
Weekes
B
Lee
KM
Soon
CS
Schreiber
A
Hoon
JJ
Chee
M
Overlap and dissociation of semantic processing of Chinese characters, English words, and pictures
Neuroimage
2000
, vol. 
12
 (pg. 
392
-
403
)
Chertkow
H
Bub
D
Deaudon
C
Whitehead
V
On the status of object concepts in aphasia
Brain Lang
1997
, vol. 
58
 (pg. 
203
-
232
)
Cipolotti
L
Butterworth
B
Denes
G
A specific deficit for numbers in a case of dense acalculia
Brain
1991
, vol. 
114
 (pg. 
2619
-
2637
)
Cohen
NJ
Eichenbaum
H
Memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal system
1993
Cambridge (MA)
MIT Press
Coltheart
M
Patterson
K
Marshall
J
Deep dyslexia
1980
London
Routledge & Kegan Paul
Conway
MA
Pleydell-Pearce
CW
The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system
Psychol Rev
2000
, vol. 
107
 (pg. 
261
-
288
)
Copland
DA
de Zubicaray
GI
McMahon
K
Wilson
SJ
Eastburn
M
Chenery
HJ
Brain activity during automatic semantic priming revealed by event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging
Neuroimage
2003
, vol. 
20
 (pg. 
302
-
310
)
Corbetta
M
Akbudak
E
Conturo
T
Snyder
AZ
Ollinger
JM
Drury
HA
Linenweber
MR
Petersen
SE
Raichle
ME
Van Essen
DC
, et al. 
A common network of functional areas for attention and eye movements
Neuron
1998
, vol. 
21
 (pg. 
761
-
773
)
Craik
FIM
Lockhart
RS
Levels of processing: a framework for memory research
J Verb Learn Verb Behav
1992
, vol. 
11
 (pg. 
671
-
684
)
Creem-Regehr
S
Lee
J
Neural representation of graspable objects: are tools special?
Cogn Brain Res
2005
, vol. 
22
 (pg. 
457
-
469
)
Critchley
M
The parietal lobes
1953
New York
Hafner
D'Esposito
M
Detre
JA
Aguirre
GK
Stallcup
M
Alsop
DC
Tippet
LJ
Farah
MJ
A functional MRI study of mental image generation
Neuropsychologia
1997
, vol. 
35
 (pg. 
725
-
730
)
Damasio
AR
Descarte's error: emotion, reason, and the human brain
1994
New York
Putnam
Damasio
H
Sarno
MT
Cerebral localization of the aphasias
Acquired aphasia
1981
Orlando
Academic Press
(pg. 
27
-
50
)
Damasio
H
Tranel
D
Grabowski
T
Adolphs
R
Damasio
A
Neural systems behind word and concept retrieval
Cognition
2004
, vol. 
92
 (pg. 
179
-
229
)
Davidoff
J
De Bleser
R
Impaired picture recognition with preserved object naming and reading
Brain Cogn
1994
, vol. 
24
 (pg. 
1
-
23
)
Davis
MH
Meunier
F
Marslen-Wilson
WD
Neural responses to morphological, syntactic, and semantic properties of single words: an fMRI study
Brain Lang
2004
, vol. 
89
 (pg. 
439
-
449
)
De Renzi
E
Liotti
M
Nichelli
P
Semantic amnesia with preservation of autobiographical memory: a case report
Cortex
1987
, vol. 
23
 (pg. 
575
-
597
)
Dehaene
S
Cohen
L
Cerebral pathways for calculation: double dissociation between rote verbal and quantitative knowledge of arithmetic
Cortex
1997
, vol. 
33
 (pg. 
219
-
250
)
Déjerine
J
Anatomie des centres nerveux
1895
Paris
Rueff
Déjerine
J
Contribution à l'étude anatomo-pathologique et clinique des différentes variétés de cécité verbale
Comptes Rend Séances Soc Biol
1892
, vol. 
44
 (pg. 
61
-
90
)
Démonet
J-F
Chollet
F
Ramsay
S
Cardebat
D
Nespoulous
J-L
Wise
R
Rascol
A
Frackowiak
R
The anatomy of phonological and semantic processing in normal subjects
Brain
1992
, vol. 
115
 (pg. 
1753
-
1768
)
Démonet
JF
Wise
R
Frackowiak
RSJ
Language functions explored in normal subjects by positron emission tomography: a critical review
Hum Brain Mapp
1993
, vol. 
1
 (pg. 
39
-
47
)
Desai
R
Conant
LL
Waldron
E
Binder
JR
FMRI of past tense processing: the effects of phonological complexity and task difficulty
J Cogn Neurosci
2006
, vol. 
18
 (pg. 
278
-
297
)
Desgranges
B
Baron
JC
Lalevee
C
Giffard
B
Viader
F
de La Sayette
V
Eustache
F
The neural substrates of episodic memory impairment in Alzheimer's disease as revealed by FDG-PET: relationship to degree of deterioration
Brain
2002
, vol. 
125
 (pg. 
1116
-
1124
)
Devlin
JT
Matthews
PM
Rushworth
MFS
Semantic processing in the left inferior prefrontal cortex: a combined functional magnetic resonance imaging and transcranial magnetic stimulation study
J Cogn Neurosci
2003
, vol. 
15
 (pg. 
71
-
84
)
Devlin
JT
Moore
CJ
Mummery
CJ
Gorno-Tempini
ML
Phillips
JA
Noppeney
U
Frackowiak
RSJ
Price
CJ
Anatomic constraints on cognitive theories of category specificity
Neuroimage
2002
, vol. 
15
 (pg. 
675
-
685
)
Devlin
JT
Russell
RP
Davis
MH
Price
CJ
Moss
HE
Jalal Fadili
M
Tyler
LK
Is there an anatomical basis for category-specificity? Semantic memory studies with PET and fMRI
Neuropsychologia
2002
, vol. 
40
 (pg. 
54
-
75
)
Drevets
WC
Price
JL
Simpson
JR
Todd
RD
Reich
T
Vannier
M
Raichle
ME
Subgenual prefrontal cortex abnormalities in mood disorders
Nature
1997
, vol. 
386
 (pg. 
824
-
827
)
Dronkers
NF
Wilkins
DP
Van Valin
RD
Redfern
BB
Jaeger
JJ
Lesion analysis of the brain areas involved in language comprehension
Cognition
2004
, vol. 
92
 (pg. 
145
-
177
)
Duhamel
JR
Colby
CL
Goldberg
ME
Ventral intraparietal area of the macaque: congruent visual and somatic response properties
J Neurophysiol
1998
, vol. 
79
 (pg. 
126
-
136
)
Duncan
J
Owen
AM
Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands
Trends Neurosci
2000
, vol. 
23
 (pg. 
475
-
483
)
Ebisch
SJH
Babiloni
C
Del Gratta
C
Ferretti
A
Perrucci
MG
Caulo
M
Sitskoorn
MM
Romani
GL
Human neural systems for conceptual knowledge of proper object use: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study
Cereb Cortex
2007
, vol. 
17
 (pg. 
2744
-
2751
)
Embick
D
Marantz
A
Miyashita
Y
O'Neil
W
Sakai
KL
A syntactic specialization for Broca's area
Proc Natl Acad Sci
2000
, vol. 
97
 (pg. 
6150
-
6154
)
Epstein
R
Kanwisher
N
A cortical representation of the local visual environment
Nature
1998
, vol. 
392
 (pg. 
598
-
601
)
Epstein
RA
Parker
WE
Feiler
AM
Where am I now? Distinct roles for parahippocampal and retrosplenial cortices in place recognition
J Neurosci
2007
, vol. 
27
 (pg. 
6141
-
6149
)
Eschen
A
Freeman
J
Dietrich
T
Martin
M
Ellis
J
Martin
E
Kliegel
M
Motor brain regions are involved in the encoding of delayed intentions: a fMRI study
Int J Psychophysiol
2007
, vol. 
64
 (pg. 
259
-
268
)
Evans
AC
Collins
DL
Mills
SR
Brown
ED
Kelly
RL
Peters
TM
3D statistical neuroanatomical models from 305 MRI volumes
IEEE nuclear science symposium and medical imaging conference
1993
(pg. 
1813
-
1817
)
Farah
MJ
Visual agnosia. Disorders of object recognition and what they tell us about normal vision
1990
Cambridge
MIT Press
Fiebach
CJ
Friederici
AD
Processing concrete words: fMRI evidence against a specific right-hemisphere involvement
Neuropsychologia
2003
, vol. 
42
 (pg. 
62
-
70
)
Fiebach
CJ
Schlesewsky
M
Lohmann
G
Revisiting the role of Broca's area in sentence processing: syntactic integration versus syntactic working memory
Hum Brain Mapp
2005
, vol. 
24
 (pg. 
79
-
91
)
Fiez
JA
Phonology, semantics and the role of the left inferior prefrontal cortex
Hum Brain Mapp
1997
, vol. 
5
 (pg. 
79
-
83
)
Fiez
JA
Balota
DA
Raichle
ME
Petersen
SE
Effects of lexicality, frequency, and spelling-to-sound consistency on the functional anatomy of reading
Neuron
1999
, vol. 
24
 (pg. 
205
-
218
)
Fletcher
PC
Happé
F
Frith
U
Baker
SC
Dolan
RJ
Frackowiak
RSJ
Frith
CD
Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension
Cognition
1995
, vol. 
57
 (pg. 
109
-
128
)
Fliessbach
K
Wesi
S
Klaver
P
Elger
CE
Weber
B
The effect of word concreteness on recognition memory
Neuroimage
2006
, vol. 
32
 (pg. 
1413
-
1421
)
Fodor
JA
The modularity of mind: an essay on faculty psychology
1983
Cambridge (MA)
MIT Press
Fogassi
L
Ferrari
PF
Gesierich
B
Rozzi
S
Chersi
F
Rizzolatti
G
Parietal lobe: from action organization to intention understanding
Science
2005
, vol. 
29
 (pg. 
662
-
667
)
Forde
EME
Humphreys
GW
Category-specific recognition impairments: a review of important case studies and influential theories
Aphasiol
1999
, vol. 
13
 (pg. 
169
-
193
)
Fox
MD
Snyder
AZ
Vincent
JL
Corbetta
M
Van Essen
DC
Raichle
ME
The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
2005
, vol. 
102
 (pg. 
9673
-
9678
)
Franklin
S
Howard
D
Patterson
K
Abstract word anomia
Cogn Neuropsychol
1995
, vol. 
12
 (pg. 
549
-
566
)
Fransson
P
Spontaneous low-frequency BOLD signal fluctuations: an fMRI investigation of the resting-state default mode of brain function hypothesis
Hum Brain Mapp
2005
, vol. 
26
 (pg. 
15
-
29
)
Freedman
M
Alexander
MP
Naeser
MA
Anatomic basis of transcortical motor aphasia
Neurology
1984
, vol. 
40
 (pg. 
409
-
417
)
Friederici
AD
Rüschemeyer
S-A
Hahne
A
Fiebach
CJ
The role of left inferior frontal gyrus and superior temporal cortex in sentence comprehension: localizing syntactic and semantic processes
Cereb Cortex
2003
, vol. 
13
 (pg. 
170
-
177
)
Frith
CD
Friston
KJ
Liddle
PF
Frackowiak
RSJ
A PET study of word finding
Neuropsychologia
1991
, vol. 
29
 (pg. 
1137
-
1148
)
Gabrieli
JDE
Poldrack
RA
Desmond
JE
The role of left prefrontal cortex in language and memory
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
1998
, vol. 
95
 (pg. 
906
-
913
)
Gainotti
G
What the locus of brain lesion tells us about the nature of the cognitive defect underlying category-specific disorders: a review
Cortex
2000
, vol. 
36
 (pg. 
539
-
559
)
Gainotti
G
Almonti
S
Di Betta
AM
Silveri
MC
Retrograde amnesia in a patient with retrosplenial tumour
Neurocase
1998
, vol. 
4
 (pg. 
519
-
526
)
Galaburda
A
Sanides
F
Cytoarchitectonic organization of the human auditory cortex
J Comp Neurol
1980
, vol. 
190
 (pg. 
597
-
610
)
Gallese
V
Lakoff
G
The brain's concepts: the role of the sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge
Cogn Neuropsychol
2005
, vol. 
22
 (pg. 
455
-
479
)
Gates
L
Yoon
MG
Distinct and shared cortical regions of the human brain activated by pictorial depictions versus verbal descriptions: an fMRI study
Neuroimage
2005
, vol. 
24
 (pg. 
473
-
486
)
Gerlach
C
A review of functional imaging studies on category specificity
J Cogn Neurosci
2007
, vol. 
19
 (pg. 
296
-
314
)
Gerlach
C
Law
I
Paulson
O
When action turns into words. Activation of motor-based knowledge during categorization of manipulable objects
J Cogn Neurosci
2002
, vol. 
14
 (pg. 
1230
-
1239
)
Gerlach
C
Law
I
Paulson
OB
Structural similarity and category-specificity: a refined account
Neuropsychologia
2004
, vol. 
42
 (pg. 
1543
-
1553
)
Gerstmann
J
Syndrome of finger agnosia, disorientation for right and left, agraphia and acalculia
Arch Neurol Psychiat
1940
, vol. 
44
 (pg. 
398
-
408
)
Geschwind
N
Aphasia
New Engl J Med
1971
, vol. 
284
 (pg. 
654
-
656
)
Geschwind
N
Disconnection syndromes in animals and man
Brain
1965
, vol. 
88
 (pg. 
237
-
294
585–644
Giambra
LM
A laboratory method for investigating influences on switching attention to task-unrelated imagery and thought
Conscious Cogn
1995
, vol. 
4
 (pg. 
1
-
21
)
Giesbrecht
B
Camblin
CC
Swaab
TY
Separable effects of semantic priming and imageability on word processing in human cortex
Cereb Cortex
2004
, vol. 
14
 (pg. 
521
-
529
)
Giraud
AL
Price
CJ
The constraints functional neuroimaging places on classical models of auditory word processing
J Cogn Neurosci
2001
, vol. 
13
 (pg. 
754
-
765
)
Gitelman
DR
Ashburner
J
Friston
KJ
Tyler
LK
Price
CJ
Voxel-based morphometry of herpes simplex encephalitis
Neuroimage
2001
, vol. 
13
 (pg. 
623
-
631
)
Gold
BT
Buckner
RL
Common prefrontal regions coactivate with dissociable posterior regions during controlled semantic and phonological tasks
Neuron
2002
, vol. 
35
 (pg. 
803
-
812
)
Goldberg
G
Supplementary motor area structure and function: review and hypothesis
Behav Brain Sci
1985
, vol. 
8
 (pg. 
567
-
616
)
Goldberg
RF
Perfetti
CA
Fiez
JA
Schneider
W
Selective retrieval of abstract semantic knowledge in left prefrontal cortex
J Neurosci
2007
, vol. 
27
 (pg. 
3790
-
3798
)
Goldberg
RF
Perfetti
CA
Schneider
W
Perceptual knowledge retrieval activates sensory brain regions
J Neurosci
2006
, vol. 
26
 (pg. 
4917
-
4921
)
Golland
Y
Bentin
S
Gelbard
H
Benjamini
Y
Heller
R
Nir
Y
Hasson
U
Malach
R
Extrinsic and intrinsic systems in the posterior cortex of the human brain revealed during natural sensory stimulation
Cereb Cortex
2007
, vol. 
17
 (pg. 
766
-
777
)
Goodglass
H
Hyde
MR
Blumstein
S
Frequency, picturability and availability of nouns in aphasia
Cortex
1969
, vol. 
5
 (pg. 
104
-
119
)
Gorno-Tempini
ML
Cipolotti
L
Price
CJ
Category differences in brain activation studies: where do they come from?
Proc Roy Soc London B
2000
, vol. 
267
 (pg. 
1253
-
1258
)
Gorno-Tempini
ML
Price
CJ
Josephs
O
Vandenberghe
R
Cappa
SF
Kapur
N
Frackowiak
RS
The neural systems sustaining face and proper-name processing
Brain
1998
, vol. 
121
 (pg. 
2103
-
2118
)
Gould
RL
Brown
RG
Owen
AM
Ffytche
DH
Howard
RJ
FMRI BOLD response to increasing task difficulty during successful paired associates learning
Neuroimage
2003
, vol. 
20
 (pg. 
1006
-
1019
)
Graham
KS
Lee
AC
Brett
M
Patterson
K
The neural basis of autobiographical and semantic memory: new evidence from three PET studies
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci
2003
, vol. 
3
 (pg. 
234
-
254
)
Graves
WW
Grabowski
TJ
Mehta
S
Gupta
P
The left posterior superior temporal gyrus participates specifically in accessing lexical phonology
J Cogn Neurosci
2008
, vol. 
20
 (pg. 
1698
-
1710
)
Grefkes
C
Fink
GR
The functional organization of the intraparietal sulcus in humans and monkeys
J Anat
2005
, vol. 
207
 (pg. 
3
-
17
)
Gregoriou
GG
Borra
E
Matelli
M
Luppino
G
Architectonic organization of the inferior parietal convexity of the macaque monkey
J Comp Neurol
2006
, vol. 
496
 (pg. 
422
-
451
)
Grodzinsky
Y
Friederici
AD
Neuroimaging of syntax and syntactic processing
Curr Opin Neurobiol
2006
, vol. 
16
 (pg. 
240
-
246
)
Grosbras
M-H
Laird
AR
Paus
T
Cortical regions involved in eye movements, shifts of attention, and gaze perception
Hum Brain Mapp
2005
, vol. 
25
 (pg. 
140
-
154
)
Grossman
M
Koenig
P
DeVita
C
Glosser
G
Alsop
D
Detre
J
Gee
J
The neural basis for category-specific knowledge: an fMRI study
Neuroimage
2002
, vol. 
15
 (pg. 
936
-
948
)
Grossman
M
Koenig
P
DeVita
C
Glosser
G
Alsop
D
Detre
J
Gee
J
Neural representation of verb meaning: an fMRI study
Hum Brain Mapp
2002
, vol. 
15
 (pg. 
124
-
134
)
Haaland
KY
Harrington
DL
Knight
RT
Neural representations of skilled movement
Brain
2000
, vol. 
123
 (pg. 
2306
-
2313
)
Hart
J
Gordon
B
Delineation of single-word semantic comprehension deficits in aphasia, with anatomic correlation
Ann Neurol
1990
, vol. 
27
 (pg. 
226
-
231
)
Hassabis
D
Kumaran
D
Maguire
EA
Using imagination to understand the neural basis of episodic memory
J Neurosci
2007
, vol. 
27
 (pg. 
14365
-
14374
)
Hasson
U
Levy
I
Behrmann
M
Hendler
T
Malach
R
Eccentricity bias as an organizing principle for human high-order object areas
Neuron
2002
, vol. 
34
 (pg. 
490
-
497
)
Hauk
O
Johnsrude
I
Pulvermuller
F
Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex
Neuron
2004
, vol. 
41
 (pg. 
301
-
307
)
Haxby
JV
Gobbini
MI
Furey
ML
Ishai
A
Schouten
JL
Pietrini
P
Distributed and overlapping representations of faces and objects in ventral temporal cortex
Science
2001
, vol. 
293
 (pg. 
2425
-
2430
)
Head
H
Aphasia and kindred disorders of speech
1926
New York
MacMillan
Heilman
KM
Bowers
D
Watson
RT
Day
A
Valenstein
E
Hammond
E
Duara
R
Frontal hypermetabolism and thalamic hypometabolism in a patient with abnormal orienting and retrosplenial amnesia
Neuropsychologia
1990
, vol. 
28
 (pg. 
161
-
169
)
Henschen
SE
On the hearing sphere
Acta Otolaryngol (Stockh)
1918
, vol. 
1
 (pg. 
423
-
486
)
Henson
RNA
Price
CJ
Rugg
MD
Turner
R
Friston
KJ
Detecting latency differences in event-related BOLD responses: application to words versus nonwords and initial versus repeated face presentations
Neuroimage
2002
, vol. 
15
 (pg. 
83
-
97
)
Hickok
G
Buchsbaum
B
Humphries
C
Muftuler
T
Auditory-motor interaction revealed by fMRI: speech, music, and working memory in area Spt
J Cogn Neurosci
2003
, vol. 
15
 (pg. 
673
-
682
)
Hillis
AE
Caramazza
A
Category-specific naming and comprehension impairment: a double dissociation
Brain
1991
, vol. 
114
 (pg. 
2081
-
2094
)
Hillis
AE
Wityk
RJ
Tuffiash
E
Beauchamp
NJ
Jacobs
MA
Barker
PB
Selnes
OA
Hypoperfusion of Wernicke's area predicts severity of semantic deficit in acute stroke
Ann Neurol
2001
, vol. 
50
 (pg. 
561
-
566
)
Ho
N
Destexhe
A
Synaptic background activity enhances the responsiveness of neocortical pyramidal neurons
J Neurophysiol
2000
, vol. 
84
 (pg. 
1488
-
1496
)
Hodges
JR
Graham
N
Patterson
K
Charting the progression in semantic dementia: implications for the organisation of semantic memory
Memory
1995
, vol. 
3
 (pg. 
463
-
495
)
Hodges
JR
Patterson
K
Oxbury
S
Funnell
E
Semantic dementia: progressive fluent aphasia with temporal lobe atrophy
Brain
1992
, vol. 
115
 (pg. 
1783
-
1806
)
Homae
F
Yahata
N
Sakai
KL
Selective enhancement of functional connectivity in the left prefrontal cortex during sentence processing
Neuroimage
2003
, vol. 
20
 (pg. 
578
-
586
)
Honey
GD
Bullmore
ET
Sharma
T
Prolonged reaction time to a verbal working memory task predicts increased power of posterior parietal cortical activation
Neuroimage
2000
, vol. 
12
 (pg. 
495
-
503
)
Horowitz
M
Intrusive and repetitive thought after experimental stress
Arch Gen Psychiatry
1975
, vol. 
32
 (pg. 
1457
-
1463
)
Humphreys
GW
Forde
EM
Hierarchies, similarity and interactivity in object recognition: ‘category specific’ neuropsychological deficits
Behav Brain Sci
2001
, vol. 
24
 (pg. 
453
-
509
)
Humphries
C
Binder
JR
Medler
DA
Liebenthal
E
Syntactic and semantic modulation of neural activity during auditory sentence comprehension
J Cogn Neurosci
2006
, vol. 
18
 (pg. 
665
-
679
)
Humphries
C
Binder
JR
Medler
DA
Liebenthal
E
Time course of semantic processes during sentence comprehension: an fMRI study
Neuroimage
2007
, vol. 
36
 (pg. 
924
-
932
)
Hyvarinen
J
Posterior parietal lobe of the primate brain
Physiol Rev
1982
, vol. 
62
 (pg. 
1060
-
1129
)
Indefrey
P
Levelt
WJM
The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components
Cognition
2004
, vol. 
92
 (pg. 
101
-
144
)
Insausti
R
Amaral
DG
Cowan
WM
The entorhinal cortex of the monkey: iI. Cortical afferents
J Comp Neurol
1987
, vol. 
264
 (pg. 
356
-
395
)
Ischebeck
A
Indefrey
P
Usui
N
Nose
I
Hellwig
F
Taira
M
Reading in a regular orthography: an fMRI study investigating the role of visual familiarity
J Cogn Neurosci
2004
, vol. 
16
 (pg. 
727
-
741
)
Ishai
A
Ungerleider
LG
Martin
A
Schouten
JL
Haxby
JV
Distributed representation of objects in the human ventral visual pathway
Proc Natl Acad Sci
1999
, vol. 
96
 (pg. 
9379
-
9384
)
James
TW
Gauthier
I
Auditory and action semantic features activate sensory-specific perceptual brain regions
Curr Biol
2003
, vol. 
13
 (pg. 
1792
-
1796
)
James
W
The stream of consciousness
Principles of psychology
1890
, vol. 
Vol. 1
 
New York
Dover Publications
(pg. 
224
-
290
)
Jax
SA
Buxbaum
LJ
Moll
AD
Deficits in movement planning and intrinsic coordinate control in ideomotor apraxia
J Cogn Neurosci
2006
, vol. 
18
 (pg. 
2063
-
2076
)
Jefferies
E
Lambon Ralph
MA
Semantic impairment in stroke aphasia versus semantic dementia: a case-series comparison
Brain
2006
, vol. 
129
 (pg. 
2132
-
2147
)
Jessen
F
Heun
R
Erb
M
Granath
DO
Klose
U
Papassotiropoulos
A
Grodd
W
The concreteness effect: evidence for dual-coding and context availability
Brain Lang
2000
, vol. 
74
 (pg. 
103
-
112
)
Johnson
MK
Foley
MA
Suengas
AG
Raye
CL
Phenomenal characteristics of memories for perceived and imagined autobiographical events
J Exp Psychol Gen
1988
, vol. 
117
 (pg. 
371
-
376
)
Johnson
MR
Mitchell
KJ
Raye
CL
D'Esposito
M
Johnson
MK
A brief thought can modulate activity in extrastriate visual areas: top-down effects of refreshing just-seen visual stimuli
Neuroimage
2007
, vol. 
37
 (pg. 
290
-
299
)
Jones
EG
Powell
TSP
An anatomical study of converging sensory pathways within the cerebral cortex of the monkey
Brain
1970
, vol. 
93
 (pg. 
793
-
820
)
Jonides
J
Schumacher
EH
Smith
EE
Lauber
E
Awh
E
Minoshima
S
Koeppe
RA
The task-load of verbal working memory affects regional brain activation as measured by PET
J Cogn Neurosci
1997
, vol. 
9
 (pg. 
462
-
475
)
Joseph
JE
Functional neuroimaging studies of category specificity in object recognition: a critical review and meta-analysis
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci
2001
, vol. 
1
 (pg. 
119
-
136
)
Kaas
JH
Hackett
TA
Subdivisions of auditory cortex and processing streams in primates
Proc Natl Acad Sci
2000
, vol. 
97
 (pg. 
11793
-
11799
)
Kable
JW
Kan
IP
Wilson
A
Thompson-Schill
SL
Chatterjee
A
Conceptual representations of action in the lateral temporal cortex
J Cogn Neurosci
2005
, vol. 
17
 (pg. 
1855
-
1870
)
Kable
JW
Lease-Spellmeyer
J
Chatterjee
A
Neural substrates for action event knowledge
J Cogn Neurosci
2002
, vol. 
14
 (pg. 
795
-
805
)
Kan
IP
Barsalou
LW
Solomon
KO
Minor
JK
Thompson-Schill
SL
Role of mental imagery in a property verification task: fMRI evidence for perceptual representations of conceptual knowledge
Cogn Neuropsychol
2003
, vol. 
20
 (pg. 
525
-
540
)
Kanwisher
N
McDermott
J
Chun
MM
The fusiform face area: a module in human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception
J Neurosci
1997
, vol. 
17
 (pg. 
4302
-
4311
)
Kapur
N
Barker
S
Burrows
EH
Ellison
D
Brice
J
Illis
LS
Scholey
K
Colbourn
C
Wilson
B
Loates
M
Herpes simplex encephalitis—long-term magnetic resonance imaging and neuropsychological profile
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
1994
, vol. 
57
 (pg. 
1334
-
1342
)
Kapur
S
Rose
R
Liddle
PF
Zipursky
RB
Brown
GM
Stuss
D
Houle
S
Tulving
E
The role of the left prefrontal cortex in verbal processing: semantic processing or willed action?
Neuroreport
1994
, vol. 
5
 (pg. 
2193
-
2196
)
Katai
S
Maruyama
T
Hashimoto
T
Yanagisawa
N
A case of cerebral infarction presenting as retrosplenial amnesia
Clin Neurol
1992
, vol. 
32
 (pg. 
1281
-
1287
)
Katz
RB
Goodglass
H
Deep dysphasia: analysis of a rare form of repetition disorder
Brain Lang
1990
, vol. 
39
 (pg. 
153
-
185
)
Kellenbach
ML
Brett
M
Patterson
K
Actions speak louder than functions: the importance of manipulability and action in tool representation
J Cogn Neurosci
2003
, vol. 
15
 (pg. 
30
-
46
)
Kertesz
A
Lau
WK
Polk
M
The structural determinants of recovery in Wernicke's aphasia
Brain Lang
1993
, vol. 
44
 (pg. 
153
-
164
)
Kertesz
A
Sheppard
A
MacKenzie
R
Localization in transcortical sensory aphasia
Arch Neurol
1982
, vol. 
39
 (pg. 
475
-
478
)
Kobayashi
Y
Amaral
DG
Macaque monkey retrosplenial cortex. II. Cortical afferents
J Comp Neurol
2003
, vol. 
466
 (pg. 
48
-
79
)
Kobayashi
Y
Amaral
DG
Macaque monkey retrosplenial cortex: III. Cortical efferents
J Comp Neurol
2007
, vol. 
502
 (pg. 
810
-
833
)
Kochunov
P
Lancaster
JL
Thompson
P
Toga
AW
Brewer
P
Hardies
J
Fox
P
An optimized individual target brain in the Talairach coordinate system
Neuroimage
2002
, vol. 
17
 (pg. 
922
-
927
)
Kounios
J
Koenig
P
Glosser
G
DeVita
C
Dennis
K
Moore
P
Grossman
M
Category-specific medial temporal lobe activation and the consolidation of semantic memory
Cogn Brain Res
2003
, vol. 
17
 (pg. 
484
-
494
)
Kuchinke
L
Jacobs
AM
Grubich
C
Vo
MLH
Conrad
M
Herrmann
M
Incidental effects of emotional valence in single word processing: an fMRI study
Neuroimage
2005
, vol. 
28
 (pg. 
1022
-
1032
)
Laine
M
Rinne
JO
Hiltunen
J
Kaasinen
V
Sipila
H
Different brain activation patterns during production of animals versus artefacts: a PET activation study on category-specific processing
Cogn Brain Res
2002
, vol. 
13
 (pg. 
95
-
99
)
Laird
AR
Fox
PM
Price
CJ
Glahn
DC
Uecker
AM
Lancaster
JL
Turkeltaub
PE
Kochunov
P
Fox
PT
ALE meta-analysis: controlling the false discovery rate and performing statistical contrasts
Hum Brain Mapp
2005
, vol. 
25
 (pg. 
155
-
164
)
Lambon Ralph
MA
Lowe
C
Rogers
TT
Neural basis of category-specific semantic deficits for living things: evidence from semantic dementia, HSVE and a neural network model
Brain
2007
, vol. 
130
 (pg. 
1127
-
1137
)
Lancaster
JL
Tordesillas-Gutierrez
D
Martinez
M
Salinas
F
Evans
AC
Zilles
K
Mazziotta
JC
Fox
PT
Bias between MNI and Talairach coordinates analyzed using the ICBM-152 brain template
Hum Brain Mapp
2007
, vol. 
28
 (pg. 
1194
-
1205
)
Lee
SS
Dapretto
M
Metaphorical vs. literal word meanings: fMRI evidence against a selective role of the right hemisphere
Neuroimage
2006
, vol. 
29
 (pg. 
536
-
544
)
Lehmann
C
Vannini
P
Wahlund
L
Almkvist
O
Dierks
T
Increased sensitivity in mapping task demand in visuospatial processing using reaction-time-dependent hemodynamic response predictors in rapid event-related fMRI
Neuroimage
2006
, vol. 
31
 (pg. 
505
-
512
)
Leichnitz
GR
An intrahemispheric columnar projection between two cortical multisensory convergence areas (inferior parietal lobule and prefrontal cortex): an anterograde study in macaque using HRP gel
Neurosci Lett
1980
, vol. 
18
 (pg. 
119
-
124
)
Levine
B
Svoboda
E
Hay
JF
Winocur
G
Moscovitch
M
Aging and autobiographical memory: dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval
Psychol Aging
2002
, vol. 
17
 (pg. 
677
-
689
)
Levine
B
Turner
GR
Tisserand
DJ
Hevenor
SJ
Graham
SJ
McIntosh
AR
The functional neuroanatomy of episodic and semantic autobiographical remembering: a prospective functional MRI study
J Cogn Neurosci
2004
, vol. 
16
 (pg. 
1633
-
1646
)
Levy
DA
Bayley
PJ
Squire
LR
The anatomy of semantic knowledge: medial vs. lateral temporal lobe
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
2004
, vol. 
101
 (pg. 
6710
-
6715
)
Liebenthal
E
Binder
JR
Spitzer
SM
Possing
ET
Medler
DA
Neural substrates of phonetic perception
Cereb Cortex
2005
, vol. 
15
 (pg. 
1621
-
1631
)
Lissauer
H
Ein fall von Seelenblindheit nebst einem Beitrage zur Theorie derselben
Arch Psychiatr Nervenkr
1889
, vol. 
21
 (pg. 
2
-
50
)
Liu
C
Hue
C
Chen
C
Chuang
K
Liang
K
Wang
Y
Wu
C
Chen
J
Dissociated roles of the middle frontal gyri in the processing of Chinese characters
Neuroreport
2006
, vol. 
17
 (pg. 
1397
-
1401
)
Logothetis
NK
Sheinberg
DL
Visual object recognition
Annu Rev Neurosci
1996
, vol. 
19
 (pg. 
577
-
621
)
Lüders
H
Lesser
RP
Hahn
J
Dinner
DS
Morris
HH
Wyllie
E
Godoy
J
Basal temporal language area
Brain
1991
, vol. 
114
 (pg. 
743
-
754
)
Luria
AR
Tsvetkova
LS
The mechanism of “dynamic aphasia”
Found Lang
1968
, vol. 
4
 (pg. 
296
-
307
)
Maddock
RJ
The retrosplenial cortex and emotion: new insights from functional neuroimaging of the human brain
Trends Neurosci
1999
, vol. 
22
 (pg. 
310
-
316
)
Maeshima
S
Kuwata
T
Masuo
O
Yamaga
H
Okita
R
Ozaki
F
Moriwaki
H
Roger
P
Transcortical sensory aphasia due to a left frontal subcortical hemorrhage
Brain Inj
1999
, vol. 
13
 (pg. 
927
-
933
)
Maeshima
S
Osawa
A
Nakayama
Y
Miki
J
Transcortical sensory aphasia following infarction in the left frontal lobe
Eur Neurol
2004
, vol. 
52
 (pg. 
125
-
128
)
Maguire
EA
Neuroimaging studies of autobiographical event memory
Phil Trans Royal Soc Lond B
2001
, vol. 
356
 (pg. 
1441
-
1451
)
Maguire
EA
Frith
CD
Lateral asymmetry in the hippocampal response to the remoteness of autobiographical memories
J Neurosci
2003
, vol. 
23
 (pg. 
5302
-
5307
)
Marr
D
Vision
1982
San Francisco
Freeman
Marshall
J
Pring
T
Robson
J
Chiat
S
When ottoman is easier than chair: an inverse frequency effect in jargon aphasia
Brain Lang
1998
, vol. 
65
 (pg. 
78
-
81
)
Martin
A
Caramazza
A
Neuropsychological and neuroimaging perspectives on conceptual knowledge: an introduction
Cogn Neuropsychol
2003
, vol. 
20
 (pg. 
195
-
212
)
Martin
A
Chao
LL
Semantic memory in the brain: structure and processes
Curr Opin Neurobiol
2001
, vol. 
11
 (pg. 
194
-
201
)
Martin
A
Haxby
JV
Lalonde
FM
Wiggs
CL
Ungerleider
LG
Discrete cortical regions associated with knowledge of color and knowledge of action
Science
1995
, vol. 
270
 (pg. 
102
-
105
)
Martin
A
Ungerleider
LG
Haxby
JV
Gazzaniga
MS
Category-specificity and the brain: the sensory-motor model of semantic representations of objects
The new cognitive neurosciences
2000
2nd ed
Cambridge (MA)
MIT Press
(pg. 
1023
-
1036
)
Martin
A
Wiggs
CL
Ungerleider
LG
Haxby
JV
Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge
Nature
1996
, vol. 
379
 (pg. 
649
-
652
)
Masdeu
JC
Schoene
WC
Funkenstein
H
Aphasia following infarction of the left supplementary motor area
Neurology
1978
, vol. 
28
 (pg. 
1220
-
1223
)
Matsumoto
A
Iidaka
T
Haneda
K
Okada
T
Sadato
N
Linking semantic priming effect in functional MRI and event-related potentials
Neuroimage
2005
, vol. 
24
 (pg. 
624
-
634
)
Mayberg
HS
Liotti
M
Brannan
SK
McGinnis
S
Mahurin
RK
Jerabek
PA
Silva
JA
Tekell
JL
Martin
CC
Reciprocal limbic-cortical function and negative mood: converging PET findings in depression and normal sadness
Am J Psychiatry
1999
, vol. 
156
 (pg. 
675
-
682
)
Mazoyer
B
Zago
L
Mellet
E
Bricogne
S
Etard
O
Houdé
O
Crivello
F
Joliot
M
Petit
L
Tzourio-Mazoyer
N
Cortical networks for working memory and executive functions sustain the conscious resting state in man
Brain Res Bull
2001
, vol. 
54
 (pg. 
287
-
298
)
McClelland
JL
McNaughton
BL
O'Reilly
RC
Why are there complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the success and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory
Psychol Rev
1995
, vol. 
102
 (pg. 
409
-
457
)
McDonald
CR
Crosson
B
Valenstein
E
Bowers
D
Verbal encoding deficits in a patient with a left retrosplenial lesion
Neurocase
2001
, vol. 
7
 (pg. 
407
-
417
)
McKiernan
KA
D'Angelo
BR
Kaufman
JN
Binder
JR
Interrupting the “stream of consciousness”: an fMRI investigation
Neuroimage
2006
, vol. 
29
 (pg. 
1185
-
1191
)
McKiernan
KA
Kaufman
JN
Kucera-Thompson
J
Binder
JR
A parametric manipulation of factors affecting task-induced deactivation in functional neuroimaging
J Cogn Neurosci
2003
, vol. 
15
 (pg. 
394
-
408
)
Mechelli
A
Gorno-Tempini
ML
Price
CJ
Neuroimaging studies of word and pseudoword reading: consistencies, inconsistencies, and limitations
J Cogn Neurosci
2003
, vol. 
15
 (pg. 
260
-
271
)
Mesulam
M
Mesulam
M
Patterns in behavioral neuroanatomy: association areas, the limbic system, and hemispheric specialization
Principles of behavioral neurology
1985
Philadelphia (PA)
F.A. Davis
(pg. 
1
-
70
)
Mesulam
MM
A cortical network for directed attention and unilateral neglect
Ann Neurol
1981
, vol. 
10
 (pg. 
309
-
325
)
Mesulam
MM
Large-scale neurocognitive networks and distributed processing for attention, language, and memory
Ann Neurol
1990
, vol. 
28
 (pg. 
597
-
613
)
Mesulam
MM
Van Hoesen
GW
Pandya
DN
Geschwind
N
Limbic and sensory connections of the inferior parietal lobule (Area PG) in the rhesus monkey: a study with a new method for horseradish peroxidase histochemistry
Brain Res
1977
, vol. 
136
 (pg. 
393
-
414
)
Mitchell
RL
The BOLD response during Stroop task-like inhibition paradigms: effects of task difficulty and task-relevant modality
Brain Cogn
2005
, vol. 
59
 (pg. 
23
-
37
)
Mohr
JP
Whitaker
H
Broca's area and Broca's aphasia
Studies in neurolinguistics
1976
New York
Academic Press
(pg. 
201
-
236
)
Moore
CJ
Price
CJ
A functional neuroimaging study of the variables that generate category specific object processing differences
Brain
1999
, vol. 
122
 (pg. 
943
-
962
)
Moore
CJ
Price
CJ
Three distinct posterior basal temporal lobe regions for reading and object naming
Neuroimage
1999
, vol. 
10
 (pg. 
181
-
192
)
Morris
R
Petrides
M
Pandya
DN
Architecture and connections of retrosplenial area 30 in the rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta)
Eur J Neurosci
1999
, vol. 
11
 (pg. 
2506
-
2518
)
Mountcastle
VB
Lynch
JC
Georgopoulos
A
Sakata
H
Acuna
C
Posterior parietal association cortex of the monkey: command functions for operations within extrapersonal space
J Neurophysiol
1975
, vol. 
38
 (pg. 
871
-
908
)
Mummery
CJ
Patterson
K
Hodges
JR
Price
CJ
Functional neuroanatomy of the semantic system: divisible by what?
J Cogn Neurosci
1998
, vol. 
10
 (pg. 
766
-
777
)
Mummery
CJ
Patterson
K
Hodges
JR
Wise
RJS
Generating ‘tiger’ as an animal name or a word beginning with T: differences in brain activation
Proc Roy Soc Lond B
1996
, vol. 
263
 (pg. 
989
-
995
)
Mummery
CJ
Patterson
K
Price
CJ
Ashburner
J
Frackowiak
RS
Hodges
JR
A voxel-based morphometry study of semantic dementia: relationship between temporal lobe atrophy and semantic memory
Ann Neurol
2000
, vol. 
47
 (pg. 
36
-
45
)
Mummery
CJ
Shallice
T
Price
CJ
Dual-process model in semantic priming: a functional imaging perspective
Neuroimage
1999
, vol. 
9
 (pg. 
516
-
525
)
Neisser
U
Cognitive psychology
1967
New York
Appleton-Century-Crofts
Nestor
PJ
Fryer
TD
Ikeda
M
Hodges
JR
Retrosplenial cortex (BA 29/30) hypometabolism in mild cognitive impairment (prodromal Alzheimer's disease)
Eur J Neurosci
2003
, vol. 
18
 (pg. 
2663
-
2667
)
Newman
SD
Just
MA
Keller
TA
Roth
J
Carpenter
PA
Differential effects of syntactic and semantic processing on the subregions of Broca's area
Cogn Brain Res
2003
, vol. 
16
 (pg. 
297
-
307
)
Ni
W
Constable
RT
Mencl
WE
Pugh
KR
Fullbright
RK
Shaywitz
SE
Shaywitz
BA
Gore
JC
An event-related neuroimaging study distinguishing form and content in sentence processing
J Cogn Neurosci
2000
, vol. 
12
 (pg. 
120
-
133
)
Noppeney
U
Friston
KJ
Price
CJ
Effects of visual deprivation on the organization of the semantic system
Brain
2003
, vol. 
126
 (pg. 
1620
-
1627
)
Noppeney
U
Josephs
O
Kiebel
S
Friston
KJ
Price
CJ
Action selectivity in parietal and temporal cortex
Cogn Brain Res
2005
, vol. 
25
 (pg. 
641
-
649
)
Noppeney
U
Patterson
K
Tyler
LK
Moss
HE
Stamatakis
EA
Bright
P
Mummery
CJ
Price
CJ
Temporal lobe lesions and semantic impairment: a comparison of herpes simplex virus encephalitis and semantic dementia
Brain
2007
, vol. 
130
 (pg. 
1138
-
1147
)
Noppeney
U
Price
CJ
Functional imaging of the semantic system: retrieval of sensory-experienced and verbally learned knowledge
Brain Lang
2003
, vol. 
84
 (pg. 
120
-
133
)
Noppeney
U
Price
CJ
Retrieval of abstract semantics
Neuroimage
2004
, vol. 
22
 (pg. 
164
-
170
)
Nyberg
L
Marklund
P
Persson
J
Cabeza
R
Forkstam
C
Petersson
K
Ingvar
M
Common prefrontal activations during working memory, episodic memory, and semantic memory
Neuropsychologia
2003
, vol. 
41
 (pg. 
371
-
377
)
Okada
T
Tanaka
S
Nakai
T
Nishizawa
S
Inui
T
Sadato
N
Yonekura
Y
Konishi
J
Naming of animals and tools: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of categorical differences in the human brain areas commonly used for naming visually presented objects
Neurosci Lett
2000
, vol. 
296
 (pg. 
33
-
36
)
O'Reilly
RC
Rudy
JW
Conjunctive representations in learning and memory: principles of cortical and hippocampal function
Psychol Rev
2001
, vol. 
108
 (pg. 
311
-
345
)
Orban
GA
Van Essen
DC
Vanduffel
W
Comparative mapping of higher visual areas in monkeys and humans
Trends Cogn Sci
2004
, vol. 
8
 (pg. 
315
-
324
)
Orfanidou
E
Marslen-Wilson
WD
Davis
MH
Neural response suppression predicts repetition priming of spoken words and pseudowords
J Cogn Neurosci
2006
, vol. 
18
 (pg. 
1237
-
1252
)
Otsuki
M
Soma
Y
Koyama
A
Yoshimura
N
Furukawa
H
Tsuji
S
Transcortical sensory aphasia following left frontal infarction
J Neurol
1998
, vol. 
245
 (pg. 
69
-
76
)
Owen
AM
McMillan
KM
Laird
AR
Bullmore
ET
N-back working memory paradigm: a meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies
Hum Brain Mapp
2005
, vol. 
25
 (pg. 
46
-
59
)
Padberg
J
Seltzer
B
Cusick
CG
Architectonics and cortical connections of the upper bank of the superior temporal sulcus in the rhesus monkey: an analysis in the tangential plane
J Comp Neurol
2003
, vol. 
467
 (pg. 
418
-
434
)
Paivio
A
A factor-analytic study of word attributes and verbal learning
J Verb Learn Verb Behav
1968
, vol. 
7
 (pg. 
41
-
49
)
Paivio
A
Mental representations: a dual-coding approach
1986
 
New York: Oxford University Press
Pandya
DN
Seltzer
B
Intrinsic connections and architectonics of the posterior parietal cortex in the rhesus monkey
J Comp Neurol
1982
, vol. 
204
 (pg. 
204
-
210
)
Parvizi
J
Van Hoesen
GW
Buckwalter
J
Damasio
A
Neural connections of the posteromedial cortex in the macaque
Proc Natl Acad Sci
2006
, vol. 
103
 (pg. 
1563
-
1568
)
Patterson
K
Nestor
PJ
Rogers
TT
Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain
Nature Rev Neurosci
2007
, vol. 
8
 (pg. 
976
-
987
)
Paulesu
E
Frith
CD
Frackowiak
RSJ
The neural correlates of the verbal component of working memory
Nature
1993
, vol. 
362
 (pg. 
342
-
345
)
Paulesu
E
McCrory
E
Fazio
F
Menoncello
L
Brunswick
N
Cappa
SF
Cotelli
M
Cossu
G
Corte
F
Lorusso
M
, et al. 
A cultural effect on brain function
Nature Neurosci
2000
, vol. 
3
 (pg. 
91
-
96
)
Penfield
W
Roberts
L
Speech and brain-mechanisms
1959
New York
Atheneum
Perani
D
Cappa
SF
Bettinardi
V
Bressi
S
Gorno-Tempini
M
Matarrese
M
Fazio
F
Different neural systems for the recognition of animals and man-made tools
Neuroreport
1995
, vol. 
6
 (pg. 
1637
-
1641
)
Perani
D
Schnur
T
Tettamanti
M
Gorno-Tempini
M
Cappa
SF
Fazio
F
Word and picture matching: a PET study of semantic category effects
Neuropsychologia
1999
, vol. 
37
 (pg. 
293
-
306
)
Petersen
SE
Fox
PT
Posner
MI
Mintun
M
Raichle
ME
Positron emission tomographic studies of the cortical anatomy of single-word processing
Nature
1988
, vol. 
331
 (pg. 
585
-
589
)
Petrides
M
Pandya
DN
Projections to the frontal cortex from the posterior parietal region in the rhesus monkey
J Comp Neurol
1984
, vol. 
228
 (pg. 
105
-
116
)
Phillips
JA
Noppeney
U
Humphreys
GW
Price
CJ
Can segregation within the semantic system account for category-specific deficits?
Brain
2002
, vol. 
125
 (pg. 
2067
-
2080
)
Phillips
ML
Drevets
WC
Rauch
SL
Lane
R
Neurobiology of emotion perception II: implications for major psychiatric disorders
Biol Psychiatry
2003
, vol. 
54
 (pg. 
515
-
528
)
Pilgrim
LK
Fadili
J
Fletcher
P
Tyler
LK
Overcoming confounds of stimulus blocking: an event-related fMRI design of semantic processing
Neuroimage
2002
, vol. 
16
 (pg. 
713
-
723
)
Poldrack
RA
Temple
E
Protopapas
A
Nagarajan
S
Tallal
P
Merzenich
MM
Gabrieli
JDE
Relations between the neural basis of dynamic auditory processing and phonological processing: evidence from fMRI
J Cogn Neurosci
2001
, vol. 
13
 (pg. 
687
-
697
)
Poldrack
RA
Wagner
AD
Prull
MW
Desmond
JE
Glover
GH
Gabrieli
JDE
Functional specialization for semantic and phonological processing in the left inferior prefrontal cortex
Neuroimage
1999
, vol. 
10
 (pg. 
15
-
35
)
Pope
KS
Singer
JL
Schwartz
GE
Shapiro
D
Regulation of the stream of consciousness: toward a theory of ongoing thought
Consciousness and self-regulation
1976
New York
Plenum Press
(pg. 
101
-
135
)
Poremba
A
Saunders
RC
Sokoloff
L
Crane
A
Cook
M
Mishkin
M
Functional mapping of the primate auditory system
Science
2003
, vol. 
299
 (pg. 
568
-
572
)
Price
CJ
Friston
KJ
Forde
EME
Humphreys
G
Functional imaging studies of category-specificity
Category specificity in brain and mind
2002
Hove, UK
Psychology Press
Price
CJ
Mummery
CJ
Moore
CJ
Frackowiak
RS
Friston
KJ
Delineating necessary and sufficient neural systems with functional imaging studies of neuropsychological patients
J Cogn Neurosci
1999
, vol. 
11
 (pg. 
371
-
382
)
Pulvermuller
F
Hauk
O
Nikulin
VV
Ilmoniemi
RJ
Functional links between motor and language systems
Eur J Neurosci
2005
, vol. 
21
 (pg. 
793
-
797
)
Raichle
ME
Neuroscience: the brain's dark energy
Science
2006
, vol. 
314
 (pg. 
1249
-
1250
)
Raichle
ME
McLeod
AM
Snyder
AZ
Powers
WJ
Gusnard
DA
Shulman
GL
A default mode of brain function
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
2001
, vol. 
98
 (pg. 
676
-
682
)
Rapcsak
SZ
Rubens
AB
Kertesz
A
Localization of lesions in transcortical aphasia
Localization and neuroimaging in neuropsychology
1994
San Diego
Academic Press
(pg. 
297
-
329
)
Reinholz
J
Pollmann
S
Differential activation of object-selective visual areas by passive viewing of pictures and words
Cogn Brain Res
2005
, vol. 
24
 (pg. 
702
-
714
)
Rissman
J
Eliassen
JC
Blumstein
SE
An event-related fMRI investigation of implicit semantic priming
J Cogn Neurosci
2003
, vol. 
15
 (pg. 
1160
-
1175
)
Rizzolatti
G
Craighero
L
The mirror–neuron system
Annu Rev Neurosci
2004
, vol. 
27
 (pg. 
169
-
192
)
Robinson
G
Blair
J
Cipolotti
L
Dynamic aphasia: an inability to select between competing verbal responses?
Brain
1998
, vol. 
121
 (pg. 
77
-
89
)
Roeltgen
DP
Sevush
S
Heilman
KM
Phonological agraphia: writing by the lexical-semantic route
Neurology
1983
, vol. 
33
 (pg. 
755
-
765
)
Roskies
AL
Fiez
JA
Balota
DA
Raichle
ME
Petersen
SE
Task-dependent modulation of regions in the left inferior frontal cortex during semantic processing
J Cogn Neurosci
2001
, vol. 
13
 (pg. 
829
-
843
)
Rossell
SL
Bullmore
ET
Williams
SCR
David
AS
Brain activation during automatic and controlled processing of semantic relations: a priming experiment using lexical-decision
Neuropsychologia
2001
, vol. 
39
 (pg. 
1167
-
1176
)
Rossell
SL
Price
CJ
Nobre
AC
The anatomy and time course of semantic priming investigated by fMRI and ERPs
Neuropsychologia
2003
, vol. 
41
 (pg. 
550
-
564
)
Rothi
LJG
Ochipa
C
Heilman
KM
A cogn neuropsycholological model of limb praxis
Cogn Neuropsychol
1991
, vol. 
8
 (pg. 
443
-
458
)
Rudge
P
Warrington
EK
Selective impairment of memory and visual perception in splenial tumours
Brain
1991
, vol. 
114
 (pg. 
349
-
360
)
Rumiati
RI
Humphreys
GW
Recognition by action: dissociating visual and semantic routes to action in normal observers
J Exp Psychol Human Percep Perform
1998
, vol. 
24
 (pg. 
631
-
647
)
Ruschmeyer
S-A
Brass
M
Friederici
AD
Comprehending prehending: neural correlates of processing verbs with motor stems
J Cogn Neurosci
2007
, vol. 
19
 (pg. 
855
-
865
)
Rushworth
MFS
Nixon
PD
Passingham
RE
Parietal cortex and movement I. Movement selection and reaching
Exp Brain Res
1997
, vol. 
117
 (pg. 
292
-
310
)
Sabsevitz
DS
Medler
DA
Seidenberg
M
Binder
JR
Modulation of the semantic system by word imageability
Neuroimage
2005
, vol. 
27
 (pg. 
188
-
200
)
Saleem
KS
Kondo
H
Price
JL
Complementary circuits connecting the orbital and medial prefrontal networks with the temporal, insular, and opercular cortex in the macaque monkey
J Comp Neurol
2007
, vol. 
506
 (pg. 
659
-
693
)
Salinas
E
Sejnowski
TJ
Correlated neuronal activity and the flow of neural information
Nature Rev Neurosci
2001
, vol. 
2
 (pg. 
539
-
550
)
Sarkissov
SA
Filimonoff
IN
Kononowa
EP
Preobraschenskaja
IS
Kukuew
LA
Atlas of the cytoarchitectonics of the human cerebral cortex
1955
Moscow
Medgiz
Schroeder
CE
Foxe
JJ
Calvert
G
Spence
C
Stein
BE
Multisensory convergence in early cortical processing
The handbook of multisensory processes
2004
Cambridge (MA)
MIT Press
(pg. 
295
-
309
)
Scott
SK
Johnsrude
IS
The neuroanatomical and functional organization of speech perception
Trends Neurosci
2003
, vol. 
26
 (pg. 
100
-
107
)
Selemon
LD
Goldman-Rakic
PS
Common cortical and subcortical targets of the dorsolateral prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices in the rhesus monkey: evidence for a distributed neural network subserving spatially guided behavior
J Neurosci
1988
, vol. 
8
 (pg. 
4049
-
4068
)
Seltzer
B
Pandya
DN
Some cortical projections to the parahippocampal gyrus in the rhesus monkey
Exp Neurol
1976
, vol. 
50
 (pg. 
146
-
160
)
Seltzer
B
Pandya
DN
Afferent cortical connections and architectonics of the superior temporal sulcus and surrounding cortex in the rhesus monkey
Brain Res
1978
, vol. 
149
 (pg. 
1
-
24
)
Seltzer
B
Pandya
DN
Further observations on parieto-temporal connections in the rhesus monkey
Exp Brain Res
1984
, vol. 
55
 (pg. 
301
-
312
)
Seltzer
B
Pandya
DN
Frontal lobe connections of the superior temporal sulcus in the rhesus monkey
J Comp Neurol
1989
, vol. 
281
 (pg. 
97
-
113
)
Seltzer
B
Pandya
DN
Parietal, temporal, and occipital projections to cortex of the superior temporal sulcus in the rhesus monkey: a retrograde tracer study
J Comp Neurol
1994
, vol. 
343
 (pg. 
445
-
463
)
Sethi
NK
Burke
L
Torgovnick
J
Arsura
E
Transcortical sensory aphasia as a result of left frontal cortical–subcortical infarction. A case report
Eur Neurol
2007
, vol. 
57
 (pg. 
52
-
53
)
Shulman
GL
Fiez
JA
Corbetta
M
Buckner
RL
Meizin
FM
Raichle
ME
Petersen
SE
Common blood flow changes across visual tasks: II. Decreases in cerebral cortex
J Cogn Neurosci
1997
, vol. 
9
 (pg. 
648
-
663
)
Simmons
A
Miller
D
Feinstein
JS
Goldberg
TE
Paulus
MP
Left inferior prefrontal cortex activation during a semantic decision-making task predicts the degree of semantic organization
Neuroimage
2005
, vol. 
28
 (pg. 
30
-
38
)
Simmons
WK
Ramjee
V
Beauchamp
MS
McRae
K
Martin
A
Barsalou
LW
A common neural substrate for perceiving and knowing about color
Neuropsychologia
2007
, vol. 
45
 (pg. 
2802
-
2810
)
Simpson
JR
Snyder
AZ
Gusnard
DA
Raichle
ME
Emotion-induced changes in human medial prefrontal cortex: I. During cognitive task performance
Proc Natl Acad Sci
2001
, vol. 
98
 (pg. 
683
-
687
)
Singer
JL
Experimental studies of ongoing conscious experience
Experimental and theoretical studies of consciousness
1993
Chichester
John Wiley & Sons
(pg. 
100
-
122
)
Small
DM
Gitelman
DR
Gregory
MD
Nobre
AC
Parrish
TB
Mesulam
MM
The posterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex mediate the anticipatory allocation of spatial attention
Neuroimage
2003
, vol. 
18
 (pg. 
633
-
641
)
Smith
EE
Jonides
J
Marshuetz
C
Koeppe
RA
Components of verbal working memory: evidence from neuroimaging
Proc Natl Acad Sci
1998
, vol. 
95
 (pg. 
876
-
882
)
Snowden
JS
Goulding
PJ
Neary
D
Semantic dementia: a form of circumscribed temporal atrophy
Behav Neurol
1989
, vol. 
2
 (pg. 
167
-
182
)
Spitsyna
G
Warren
JE
Scott
SK
Turkheimer
FE
Wise
RJS
Converging language streams in the human temporal lobe
J Neurosci
2006
, vol. 
26
 (pg. 
7328
-
7336
)
Stark
CE
Squire
LR
When zero is not zero: the problem of ambiguous baseline conditions in fMRI
Proc Natl Acad Sci
2001
, vol. 
98
 (pg. 
12760
-
12766
)
Suzuki
WA
Amaral
DG
Perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices of the macaque monkey: cortical afferents
J Comp Neurol
1994
, vol. 
350
 (pg. 
497
-
533
)
Svoboda
E
McKinnon
MC
Levine
B
The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: a meta-analysis
Neuropsychologia
2006
, vol. 
44
 (pg. 
2189
-
2208
)
Takayama
Y
Kamo
H
Ohkawa
Y
Akiguchi
I
Kimura
J
A case of retrosplenial amnesia
Brain
1991
, vol. 
110
 (pg. 
331
-
333
)
Talairach
J
Tournoux
P
Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain
1988
New York
Thieme Medical Publishers
Tan
LH
Laird
AR
Li
K
Fox
PT
Neuroanatomical correlates of phonological processing of Chinese characters and alphabetic words: a meta-analysis
Hum Brain Mapp
2005
, vol. 
25
 (pg. 
83
-
91
)
Tanaka
K
Saito
H
Fukada
Y
Moriya
M
Coding visual images of objects in the inferotemporal cortex of the macaque monkey
J Neurophysiol
1991
, vol. 
66
 (pg. 
170
-
189
)
Teasdale
JD
Dritschel
BH
Taylor
MJ
Proctor
L
Lloyd
CA
Nimmo-Smith
I
Baddeley
AD
Stimulus-independent thought depends on central executive resources
Mem Cognit
1995
, vol. 
23
 (pg. 
551
-
559
)
Teasdale
JD
Proctor
L
Lloyd
CA
Baddeley
AD
Working memory and stimulus-independent thought: effects of memory load and presentation rate
Eur J Cogn Psychol
1993
, vol. 
5
 (pg. 
417
-
433
)
Tettamanti
M
Buccino
G
Saccuman
MC
Gallese
V
Danna
M
Scifo
P
Fazio
F
Rizzolatti
G
Cappa
SF
Perani
D
Listening to action-related sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits
J Cogn Neurosci
2005
, vol. 
17
 (pg. 
273
-
281
)
Thioux
M
Pesenti
M
Costes
N
De Volder
A
Seron
X
Task-independent semantic activation for numbers and animals
Cogn Brain Res
2005
, vol. 
24
 (pg. 
284
-
290
)
Thompson-Schill
SL
Neuroimaging studies of semantic memory: inferring “how” from “where”
Neuropsychologia
2003
, vol. 
41
 (pg. 
280
-
292
)
Thompson-Schill
SL
Aguirre
GK
D'Esposito
M
Farah
MJ
A neural basis for category and modality specificity of semantic knowledge
Neuropsychologia
1999
, vol. 
37
 (pg. 
671
-
676
)
Thompson-Schill
SL
D'Esposito
M
Aguirre
GK
Farah
MJ
Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: a reevaluation
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
1997
, vol. 
94
 (pg. 
14792
-
14797
)
Thompson-Schill
SL
D'Esposito
M
Kan
IP
Effects of repetition and competition on activity in left prefrontal cortex during word generation
Neuron
1999
, vol. 
23
 (pg. 
513
-
522
)
Tomasino
B
Werner
C
Weiss
PH
Fink
GR
Stimulus properties matter more than perspective: an fMRI study of mental imagery and silent reading of action phrases
Neuroimage
2007
, vol. 
36
 (pg. 
T128
-
T141
)
Tootell
RB
Reppas
JB
Kwong
KK
Malach
R
Born
RT
Brady
TJ
Rosen
BR
Belliveau
JW
Functional analysis of human MT and related visual cortical areas using magnetic resonance imaging
J Neurosci
1995
, vol. 
15
 (pg. 
3215
-
3230
)
Tranel
D
Damasio
H
Damasio
AR
A neural basis for the retrieval of conceptual knowledge
Neuropsychologia
1997
, vol. 
35
 (pg. 
1319
-
1327
)
Tranel
D
Kemmerer
D
Adolphs
R
Damasio
H
Damasio
AR
Neural correlates of conceptual knowledge for actions
Cogn Neuropsychol
2003
, vol. 
20
 (pg. 
409
-
432
)
Tregallas
JR
Davalos
DB
Rojas
DC
Effect of task difficulty on the functional anatomy of temporal processing
Neuroimage
2006
, vol. 
32
 (pg. 
307
-
315
)
Tulving
E
Tulving
E
Donaldson
W
Episodic and semantic memory
Organization of memory
1972
New York
Academic Press
(pg. 
381
-
403
)
Turkeltaub
PE
Eden
GF
Jones
KM
Zeffiro
TA
Meta-analysis of the functional neuroanatomy of single-word reading: method and validation
Neuroimage
2002
, vol. 
16
 (pg. 
765
-
780
)
Tyler
LK
Bright
P
Dick
P
Tavares
P
Pilgrim
LK
Fletcher
P
Do semantic categories activate distinct cortical regions? Evidence for a distributed neural semantic system
Cogn Neuropsychol
2003
, vol. 
20
 (pg. 
541
-
559
)
Tyler
LK
Russell
R
Fadili
J
Moss
HE
The neural representation of nouns and verbs: PET studies
Brain
2001
, vol. 
124
 (pg. 
1619
-
1634
)
Tyler
LK
Stamatakis
EA
Dick
E
Bright
P
Fletcher
P
Moss
HE
Objects and their actions: evidence for a neurally distributed semantic system
Neuroimage
2003
, vol. 
18
 (pg. 
542
-
557
)
Ullsperger
M
von Cramon
DY
Subprocesses of performance monitoring: a dissociation of error processing and response competition revealed by event-related fMRI and ERPs
Neuroimage
2001
, vol. 
14
 (pg. 
1387
-
1401
)
Valenstein
E
Bowers
D
Verfaellie
M
Heilman
KM
Day
A
Watson
RT
Retrosplenial amnesia
Brain
1987
, vol. 
110
 (pg. 
1631
-
1646
)
Van Hoesen
GW
The parahippocampal gyrus: new observations regarding its cortical connections in the monkey
Trends Neurosci
1982
, vol. 
5
 (pg. 
345
-
350
)
van Veen
V
Carter
CS
The anterior cingulate as a conflict monitor: fMRI and ERP studies
Physiol Behav
2002
, vol. 
77
 (pg. 
477
-
482
)
Vandenbulcke
M
Peeters
R
Fannes
K
Vandenberghe
R
Knowledge of visual attributes in the right hemisphere
Nature Neurosci
2006
, vol. 
9
 (pg. 
964
-
970
)
Varney
NR
Damasio
H
Locus of lesion in impaired pantomime recognition
Cortex
1987
, vol. 
23
 (pg. 
699
-
703
)
Vigneau
M
Beaucousin
V
Herve
PY
Duffau
H
Crivello
F
Houde
O
Mazoyer
B
Tzourio-Mazoyer
N
Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: phonology, semantics, and sentence processing
Neuroimage
2006
, vol. 
30
 (pg. 
1414
-
1432
)
Vincent
JL
Snyder
AZ
Fox
MD
Shannon
BJ
Andrews
JR
Raichle
ME
Buckner
RL
Coherent spontaneous activity identifies a hippocampal–parietal memory network
J Neurophysiol
2006
, vol. 
96
 (pg. 
3517
-
3531
)
Vogt
BA
Pandya
DN
Cingulate cortex of the rhesus monkey: II. Cortical afferents
J Comp Neurol
1987
, vol. 
262
 (pg. 
271
-
289
)
Vogt
BA
Vogt
L
Laureys
S
Cytology and functionally correlated circuits of human posterior cingulate areas
Neuroimage
2006
, vol. 
29
 (pg. 
452
-
466
)
Vogt
C
Vogt
O
Allgemeinere Erg ebrisse unserer Hirnforschung
J Psychol Neurol
1919
, vol. 
25
 (pg. 
279
-
461
)
von Bonin
G
The evolution of the human brain
1962
Chicago
University of Chicago Press
von Bonin
G
Bailey
P
The neocortex of macaca mulatta
1947
Urbana (IL)
University of Illinois Press
von Economo
K
Koskinas
G
Die Cytoarchitektonik der Hirnrinde des erwachsenen Menschen
1925
Vienna
Springer
von Kriegstein
K
Eger
E
Kleinschmidt
A
Giraud
AL
Modulation of neural responses to speech by directing attention to voices or verbal content
Cogn Brain Res
2003
, vol. 
17
 (pg. 
48
-
55
)
Wagner
AD
Koutstaal
W
Maril
A
Schacter
DL
Buckner
RL
Task-specific repetition priming in left inferior prefrontal cortex
Cereb Cortex
2000
, vol. 
10
 (pg. 
1176
-
1184
)
Wagner
AD
Pare-Blagoev
EJ
Clark
J
Poldrack
RA
Recovering meaning: left prefrontal cortex guides semantic retrieval
Neuron
2001
, vol. 
31
 (pg. 
329
-
338
)
Wallentin
M
Lund
TE
Ostergaard
S
Ostergaard
L
Roepstorff
A
Motion verb sentences activate left posterior middle temporal cortex despite static context
Neuroreport
2005
, vol. 
16
 (pg. 
649
-
652
)
Wallentin
M
Østergaarda
S
Lund
TE
Østergaard
L
Roepstorff
A
Concrete spatial language: see what I mean?
Brain Lang
2005
, vol. 
92
 (pg. 
221
-
233
)
Warrington
EK
Frederiks
JAM
Agnosia: the impairment of object recognition
Handbook of clinical neurology
1985
New York
Elsevier
(pg. 
333
-
349
)
Warrington
EK
Concrete word dyslexia
Br J Psychol
1981
, vol. 
72
 (pg. 
175
-
196
)
Warrington
EK
The selective impairment of semantic memory
Q J Exp Psychol
1975
, vol. 
27
 (pg. 
635
-
657
)
Warrington
EK
McCarthy
RA
Categories of knowledge. Further fractionations and an attempted integration
Brain
1987
, vol. 
110
 (pg. 
1273
-
1296
)
Warrington
EK
McCarthy
RA
Multiple meaning systems in the brain: a case for visual semantics
Neuropsychologia
1994
, vol. 
32
 (pg. 
1465
-
1473
)
Warrington
EK
Shallice
T
Category specific semantic impairments
Brain
1984
, vol. 
107
 (pg. 
829
-
854
)
Wernicke
C
Der aphasische Symptomenkomplex
1874
Breslau
Cohn & Weigert
Whatmough
C
Chertkow
H
Murtha
S
Hanratty
K
Dissociable brain regions process object meaning and object structure during picture naming
Neuropsychologia
2002
, vol. 
40
 (pg. 
174
-
186
)
Whatmough
C
Verret
L
Fung
D
Chertkow
H
Common and contrasting areas of activation for abstract and concrete concepts: an H2 15O PET study
J Cogn Neurosci
2004
, vol. 
16
 (pg. 
1211
-
1226
)
Wheatley
T
Weisberg
J
Beauchamp
MS
Martin
A
Automatic priming of semantically related words reduces activity in the fusiform gyrus
J Cogn Neurosci
2005
, vol. 
17
 (pg. 
1871
-
1885
)
Wible
CG
Han
SD
Spencer
MH
Kubicki
M
Niznikiewicz
MH
Jolesz
FA
McCarley
RW
Nestor
P
Connectivity among semantic associates: an fMRI study of semantic priming
Brain Lang
2006
, vol. 
97
 (pg. 
294
-
305
)
Wise
RJS
Howard
D
Mummery
CJ
Fletcher
P
Leff
A
Büchel
C
Scott
SK
Noun imageability and the temporal lobes
Neuropsychologia
2000
, vol. 
38
 (pg. 
985
-
994
)
Wise
RSJ
Scott
SK
Blank
SC
Mummery
CJ
Murphy
K
Warburton
EA
Separate neural subsystems within ‘Wernicke's area’
Brain
2001
, vol. 
124
 (pg. 
83
-
95
)
Xiao
Z
Zhang
JX
Wang
X
Wu
R
Hu
X
Weng
X
Tan
LH
Differential activity in left inferior frontal gyrus for pseudowords and real words: an event-related fMRI study on auditory lexical decision
Hum Brain Mapp
2005
, vol. 
25
 (pg. 
212
-
221
)
Xu
J
Kemeny
S
Park
G
Frattali
C
Braun
A
Language in context: emergent features of word, sentence, and narrative comprehension
Neuroimage
2005
, vol. 
25
 (pg. 
1002
-
1015
)
Yasuda
K
Wattanabe
O
Ono
Y
Dissociation between semantic and autobiographical memory: a case report
Cortex
1997
, vol. 
33
 (pg. 
623
-
638
)
Yasuno
F
Nishikawa
T
Tokunaga
H
Yoshiyama
K
Nakagawa
Y
Ikejiri
Y
Oku
N
Hashikawa
K
Tanabe
H
Shinozaki
K
, et al. 
The neural basis of perceptual and conceptual word priming – a PET study
Cortex
2000
, vol. 
36
 (pg. 
59
-
69
)
Zatorre
RJ
Evans
AC
Meyer
E
Gjedde
A
Lateralization of phonetic and pitch discrimination in speech processing
Science
1992
, vol. 
256
 (pg. 
846
-
849
)

Supplementary data