Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2010 Feb 2;107(5):2367-72.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.0911725107. Epub 2010 Jan 19.

Running enhances spatial pattern separation in mice

Affiliations

Running enhances spatial pattern separation in mice

David J Creer et al. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. .

Abstract

Increasing evidence suggests that regular exercise improves brain health and promotes synaptic plasticity and hippocampal neurogenesis. Exercise improves learning, but specific mechanisms of information processing influenced by physical activity are unknown. Here, we report that voluntary running enhanced the ability of adult (3 months old) male C57BL/6 mice to discriminate between the locations of two adjacent identical stimuli. Improved spatial pattern separation in adult runners was tightly correlated with increased neurogenesis. In contrast, very aged (22 months old) mice had impaired spatial discrimination and low basal cell genesis that was refractory to running. These findings suggest that the addition of newly born neurons may bolster dentate gyrus-mediated encoding of fine spatial distinctions.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Figures

Fig. 1.
Fig. 1.
Mice were trained to distinguish between arrays of stimuli displayed on a touch screen in an operant chamber. (A) Three stimulus arrays were used during the experiments. (i) Mice were initially trained with an intermediate separation between stimuli for task training. Thereafter, animals started probe sessions with big (ii) and small (iii) stimulus separation conditions. (B) Mice were housed with or without a running wheel. (C) Timeline of the study.
Fig. 2.
Fig. 2.
Acquisition of big and small separation between stimuli. Mice were trained to reach a criterion of seven of eight trials correct. (A) Runners (run) performed better than controls (con) in acquisition in the small separation condition (*P < 0.05) but not in the big separation condition (P > 0.24). (B) Trend toward a correlation between trials to acquisition of the small separation and newly born neuron density was observed (P = 0.13). Photomicrographs of BrdU-positive cells in control (C) and runner (D) dentate gyrus 10 weeks after the last injection. (Scale bar: 50 μm)
Fig. 3.
Fig. 3.
Reversal of stimulus reinforcement in the big and small conditions. The reinforcement of the stimuli was reversed each time a mouse reached criterion so that the previously correct location became the incorrect location and vice versa. (A) There was no difference between the groups in the big separation condition in the number of reversals. con, control; run, runner. (B) Runners performed better than controls in the reversal task when the separation between stimuli was small (*P < 0.002). (C) Significant correlation between task performance in the small condition and newly born neuron density was observed (P < 0.03). Confocal images of BrdU-positive cells in the dentate gyrus of sections derived from control (D) and runner (E) mice 10 weeks after the last injection. Sections were immunofluorescent double-labeled for BrdU (green) and NeuN (red). (Scale bar: 50 μm) (F) Newly born neuron density was increased in runners as compared with controls (*P < 0.0001).
Fig. 4.
Fig. 4.
Intermediate spatial pattern separation in aged mice. Mice (n = 8) were trained for 10 days under sedentary conditions (block 1). Thereafter, mice were divided into sedentary (n = 4) and runner (n = 4) groups, injected with BrdU over 5 days, and trained for an additional 10 days on the same task (block 2). (A) Running improved performance on acquisition of the task in block 2 as compared with block 1 (P < 0.03). (B) Reversal performance was not influenced by exercise. con, control; run, runner. The number of BrdU-labeled cells did not differ between controls (C) and runners (D) at 17 days. (E and F) Lectin-stained vessel density was quantified using imaging software. There was no change in dentate gyrus vasculature in running aged mice (F) as compared with controls (E). (Scale bar: 100 μm.)

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. Sibley BA, Etnier JL. The relationship between physical activity and cognition in children: A meta-analysis. Pediatric Exercise Science. 2003;15:243–256.
    1. Hillman CH, Erickson KI, Kramer AF. Be smart, exercise your heart: Exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9:58–65. - PubMed
    1. Gazzaley A, Sheridan MA, Cooney JW, D’Esposito M. Age-related deficits in component processes of working memory. Neuropsychology. 2007;21:532–539. - PubMed
    1. Colcombe SJ, et al. Aerobic fitness reduces brain tissue loss in aging humans. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2003;58:176–180. - PubMed
    1. Fordyce DE, Farrar RP. Enhancement of spatial learning in F344 rats by physical activity and related learning-associated alterations in hippocampal and cortical cholinergic functioning. Behav Brain Res. 1991;46:123–133. - PubMed

Publication types

Substances

LinkOut - more resources

-