Skip to main page content
U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

Dot gov

The .gov means it’s official.
Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

Https

The site is secure.
The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
. 2023 Jul 21;14(1):25.
doi: 10.1186/s13229-023-00556-3.

Rhesus macaque social functioning is paternally, but not maternally, inherited by sons: potential implications for autism

Affiliations

Rhesus macaque social functioning is paternally, but not maternally, inherited by sons: potential implications for autism

Joseph P Garner et al. Mol Autism. .

Abstract

Background: Quantitative autistic traits are common, heritable, and continuously distributed across the general human population. Patterns of autistic traits within families suggest that more complex mechanisms than simple Mendelian inheritance-in particular, parent of origin effects-may be involved. The ideal strategy for ascertaining parent of origin effects is by half-sibling analysis, where half-siblings share one, but not both, parents and each individual belongs to a unique combination of paternal and maternal half-siblings. While this family structure is rare in humans, many of our primate relatives, including rhesus macaques, have promiscuous breeding systems that consistently produce paternal and maternal half-siblings for a given index animal. Rhesus macaques, like humans, also exhibit pronounced variation in social functioning.

Methods: Here we assessed differential paternal versus maternal inheritance of social functioning in male rhesus macaque offspring (N = 407) using ethological observations and ratings on a reverse-translated quantitative autistic trait measurement scale. Restricted Maximum Likelihood mixed models with unbounded variance estimates were used to estimate the variance components needed to calculate the genetic contribution of parents as the proportion of phenotypic variance (σ2P) between sons that could uniquely be attributed to their shared genetics (σ2g), expressed as σ2g2P (or the proportion of phenotypic variance attributable to genetic variance), as well as narrow sense heritability (h2).

Results: Genetic contributions and heritability estimates were strong and highly significant for sons who shared a father but weak and non-significant for sons who shared a mother. Importantly, these findings were detected using the same scores from the same sons in the same analysis, confirmed when paternal and maternal half-siblings were analyzed separately, and observed with two methodologically distinct behavioral measures. Finally, genetic contributions were similar for full-siblings versus half-siblings that shared only a father, further supporting a selective paternal inheritance effect.

Limitations: These data are correlational by nature. A larger sample that includes female subjects, enables deeper pedigree assessments, and supports molecular genetic analyses is warranted.

Conclusions: Rhesus macaque social functioning may be paternally, but not maternally, inherited by sons. With continued investigation, this approach may yield important insights into sex differences in autism's genetic liability.

Keywords: Autism spectrum disorder; Autistic traits; Heritability; Parent of origin effect; Primate model; Rhesus macaque; Social Responsiveness Scale; Social functioning.

PubMed Disclaimer

Conflict of interest statement

The authors declare no competing interests in relation to the work described.

Figures

Fig. 1
Fig. 1
Pedigrees depict the three types of sibling groups and parental genetic contributions to sons’ social functioning. Paternal and maternal genetic contribution was estimated as σ2g2P, or the proportion of phenotypic variance attributable to genetic variance. Sons are depicted by small blue squares, fathers by large blue squares, and mothers by large orange circles. a Non-social behavior for paternal and maternal half-siblings. b Non-social behavior for full-siblings. c Quantitative autistic-like traits for paternal and maternal half-siblings. Of the N = 24 full-siblings in panel b, the eldest in each pair is included in the half-sibling analysis in panel a. Of the N = 274 half-siblings in panel a, N = 175 are included in the analysis in panel c. Similarly, of the N = 194 half-siblings in panel c, N = 175 are included in the analysis in panel a. A selective paternal transmission effect was found for both social functioning measures
Fig. 2
Fig. 2
Parental genetic contributions to sons’ non-social behavior and quantitative autistic-like trait variation. The genetic contribution to sons’ a non-social scores and b mSRS-R scores is estimated as the BLUP for each parent. Each symbol represents the mean deviation of the sons within a half-sibling group descended from an individual parent, plotted on the x-axis in an arbitrary order. The wider range of these values in fathers (blue) versus mothers (orange) is highly significant for each behavior measure and leads to the greater σ2g2P and h2 estimates reported in the text. Sample sizes for a were N = 89 fathers and N = 237 mothers and for b were N = 69 fathers and N = 171 mothers. Non-social behavior was measured using slightly different sampling methods between cohorts and was accordingly Z-scored within each cohort to produce the “non-social equivalence score” shown here, whereas mSRS-R scores were measured with the same scale across cohorts and thus did not require transformation

Similar articles

Cited by

References

    1. APA . Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.
    1. Constantino JN. The quantitative nature of autistic social impairment. Pediatr Res. 2011;69(5 Pt 2):55R–62R. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e318212ec6e. - DOI - PMC - PubMed
    1. Pickles A, Starr E, Kazak S, Bolton P, Papanikolaou K, Bailey A, et al. Variable expression of the autism broader phenotype: findings from extended pedigrees. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2000;41(4):491–502. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00634. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Piven J, Palmer P, Jacobi D, Childress D, Arndt S. Broader autism phenotype: evidence from a family history study of multiple-incidence autism families. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154(2):185–190. doi: 10.1176/ajp.154.2.185. - DOI - PubMed
    1. Bishop DV, Maybery M, Maley A, Wong D, Hill W, Hallmayer J. Using self-report to identify the broad phenotype in parents of children with autistic spectrum disorders: a study using the Autism-Spectrum Quotient. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2004;45(8):1431–1436. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00325.x. - DOI - PubMed

Publication types

-