Skip to main content
Access keys NCBI Homepage MyNCBI Homepage Main Content Main Navigation
Ann Neurosci. 2012 Oct; 19(4): 165–168.
PMCID: PMC4117054
PMID: 25205992

Gender based variation in cognitive functions in adolescent subjects

Abstract

Background

Cognition can be defined as all mental activities that are involved in acquisition, processing, storage and retrieval of information.

Purpose

There is paucity of the data related to cognitive function amongst healthy adolescent age group which limits our ability to distinguish and compare cognitive changes that occur across the adult lifespan in female and male subjects separately and can provide some help to understand dementia related conditions.

Methods

Cognitive function was assessed in 100 healthy subjects of each sex of 17–20 years by using ‘Montreal Cognition Assessment Test’, a 10 minutes: 30-points test which is used in assessing a wide range of cognitive abilities on 7 subscales : 1) Visuospatial Skills, 2) Language, 3) Memory, 4) Attention, 5) Mathematical ability, 6) Abstraction, and 7) Orientation.

Results

Overall score (Male: 25.16 ± 1.8, Female: 25.72 ± 1.8) of cognition functions was statistically significantly higher in female adolescents (p=<0.02). However male subjects showed higher score in Mathematical ability.

Conclusion

There are variations in the cognitive functions in male and female individuals and neuroanatomical and physiological differences contribute to these variations.

Keywords: Gender, Cognition, MOCA, Prefrontal cortex, Memory

Introduction

The prefrontal cortex is important for a wide range of cognitive functions. Cognition can be defined as all mental activities that are involved in acquisition, processing, storage and retrieval of information. It includes a variety of skills attention, learning, memory, verbal ability or language, visuospatial function, and a group of abilities, known as executive function i.e. reasoning, abstraction and mental flexibility.1 There is paucity of the data related to cognitive function amongst healthy adolescent age group which limits our ability to distinguish and compare cognitive changes that occur across the adult lifespan in female and male subjects separately and can provide some help to understand dementia related conditions.

Methods

Study was carried out in the department of Physiology, SGRRIM&HS, Dehradun on 200 mentally and physically healthy volunteers (medical students) of both sexes of 17–19 years after seeking written informed consent and getting permission from the institutional ethical committee for the study. Montreal Cognition Assessment Test’ (MOCA),2 a 10 min: 30-points test, was undertaken to score cognitive abilities on 7 subtests i) Visuo-spatial Skills, ii) Language, iii) Memory, iv) Attention, v) Mathematical ability, vi) Abstraction, vii) Orientation.

  1. Visuospatial Skills: 5 points
    1. Alternating trail making (1 point)
    2. Visuoconstruction skills (two exercise 1and 3 points)
  2. Language: 6 points
    1. Naming the figures (3 points)
    2. Sentence repeating (2 points)
    3. Verbal fluency (1 point) at least >11 words with any letter
  3. Memory: 5 points

    Recalling 5 different (not related) words

  4. Attention: 3 points
    1. Forward digit span
    2. Backward digit span and
    3. Vigilance: tap only on saying specific letter.

Mathematical ability: 3 points

Serial 7s substraction

Abstraction: 2 points

Similarities in the pair of words, eg Train-Bicycle

Orientation: 6 points

a) day, b) month, c) year, d) place and e) city

Interpretation: On sum of scoring points if score is < 21: Cognitive impairment.

Group A - Male adolescents

Group B - Female adolescents

Statistical Analyses

All statistical Analyses were performed using the SPSS version 17-0 programme.

  • Univariate analysis of variance was done to compare any statistically significant difference in overall score of cognitive functions of both the sexes.
  • One way anova was done to compare any statistically significant difference between various subtests of cognitive functions of both the sexes.

Results

Sum score of all the subtests together of ‘30 – point MOCA test’ for Group 2 was higher (25.21) in comparison to Group1 (25.01) (Table 1). Univariate analysis of variance showed statistically significantly difference in cognition functions between two groups (F1,399 = 1604.29, p = <0.001) (Table 2). The mean scores of individual subtests for Group 1 and Group 2 are given in Table 1. One Way Anova test showed statistically significant differnce between two groups for all the subtest of cognition functions (Table 3). It was found that Group 2 has statistically significantly higher scores in visuospatial skill and memory in comparison to that in Group 1(visuospatial; F1,199 = 39.365, p =<0.001; Memory ; F1,199 = 4.304, p = <0.05). On the other hand Group 1 has statistically significantly higher scores in mathematical ability (F1,199 = 48.41, p = <0.001). The values for language and attention were higher in female subjects in comparison to males but these were statistically insignificant (Language; F1,199 = 0.103, p = >0.05; Attention ; F1,199 = 0.139, p = >0.05). The values for abstraction and orientation were similar in both the groups (Table 1).

Table 1:

’Montreal Cognition Assessment Test’ (MoCA Test) Score on 7 subscales in two groups
Descriptives NMeanStd.
Deviation
Std.
Error
Visuospatial
(5 points)
Group 11003.480.560.06
Group 21004.050.720.07
Total2003.7650.700.05
Language
(6 points)
Group 11004.80.820.08
Group 21004.840.940.09
Total2004.820.880.06
Memory
(5 points)
Group 11003.850.870.09
Group 21004.10.830.08
Total2003.9750.860.06
Attention
(3 points)
Group 11002.460.830.08
Group 21002.50.670.07
Total2002.480.760.05
Mathematical
(3 points)
Group 11002.80.600.06
Group 21002.120.770.08
Total2002.460.770.05
Abstraction
(2 points)
Group 11001.620.490.05
Group 21001.60.490.05
Total2001.610.490.03
Orientation
(6 points)
Group 110060.000.00
Group 210060.000.00
Total20060.000.00
Sum of all the MEAN values of 7 subtests of both groups
Overall Score
(30 points)
Group 170025.01.........
Group 270025.21........

Table 2:

Univariate Analysis of Variance. Comparison of cognitive functions between two Groups
SourceType III
Sum of
Squares
dfMean
Square
FSig.
a. R Squared = .003 (Adjusted R Squared = .002)
Corrected
Model
11.092a25.5462.2440.106
Intercept3964.54613964.551604.2950.000
Cognitive
functions
10.806110.8064.3730.037
Groups0.28610.2860.1160.734
Error3452.27613972.471  
Total214781400   
Corrected
Total
3463.3691399   

Table 3:

ANOVA Test for the comparison of the cognitive functions of both the sexes
  Sum of SquaresdfMean SquareFSig.
VisuospatialBetween Groups16.245116.24539.3650
Within Groups81.711980.413  
Total97.955199   
LanguageBetween Groups0.0810.080.1030.748
Within Groups153.441980.775  
Total153.52199   
MemoryBetween Groups3.12513.1254.3040.039
Within Groups143.751980.726  
Total146.875199   
AttentionBetween Groups0.0810.080.1390.71
Within Groups113.841980.575  
Total113.92199   
MathematicalBetween Groups23.12123.1248.4110
Within Groups94.561980.478  
Total117.68199   
AbstractionBetween Groups0.0210.020.0830.773
Within Groups47.561980.24  
Total47.58199   
OrientationBetween Groups010..
Within Groups01980  
Total0199   

Discussion

The results of the present study show statistically significantly higher overall cognitive score in female young adults in comparison to male young adults. In earlier related studies, difference in overall level of cognition amongst the two sexes was not found in the studies of Kimura D, Herlitz A, Halpern DF, Downing K & Voyer E as they proclaim that most of the standard tests have been shaped to avoid gender bias, although almost all of these gender based studies quote differences in the various components of cognitive abilities.37

Out of 7 subsets of Cognition test, since we obtained higher scores in 4 subtests in female subjects, 2 equal scores and 1 higher score in male subjects thus obviously raising the overall score in female subjects (fig 1). Above findings in our study may also be attributed to greater body-brain weight ratio for the female although male brain is 10% larger8. Further, MRI studies have revealed that the brain in female subjects contain relatively greater proportion of gray mater9,10. Presently, it is an observed fact that in many medical institutes female students surpass males in the scores obtained in various levels of examination systems.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is ANS0972-7531-19-165-g001.jpg

Gender wise comparison of cognition

The findings of our study revealed that females excelled in visuospatial skills, language, memory, attention subtests for cognition and statistically significant results were found for visuospatial skills and memory. Earlier many studies have revealed that men performed at a sizeably higher level on most visuospatial tasks than women 36,11,12 and these were correlated to the influence of sex hormone levels (testosterone). It was said that men with higher levels of endogenous testosterone were expected to perform better than men with lower levels and the cause was postulated to be linked to the presence of receptors in the certain brain areas1316 but some investigators mentioned non-significant association to androgens.17,18 Further, it appears that early prenatal presence of androgens may organize the male brain to enhance certain spatial functions. Higher level of testosterone can be taken to be conclusive in affecting the spatial ability by the results of studies on females, suffering from congenital adrenal hyperplasia19 showing better visuospatial skills. On the other hand estrogen was thought to have an inhibitory influence on visuospatial skills.20

On the contrary, the better visuospatial skill in our study in female subjects can be explained on the basis of changing trend of the technologies, indulgence and predilection of females since very early childhood. Currently, even girls play video games, handle computers and this makes them proficient in tackling all sorts of images orientation. The female subjects included in our study were the ones having admittance to new advent of computer era since early childhood.

Our findings of better memory in females were in accordance to the earlier studies of Delis and Chipman et al.21,22 A significant finding by Kimura D also suggested that women excel on a test of ‘location memory for objects.3 Estrogen is thought to have a facilitating effect on tasks of verbal memory in which women typically excel, such as articulation, speed, and coordination. Role of estrogen can further be supported by the studies conducted during midluteal phase of the menstrual cycle and women perform at a higher level on these tasks, when estrogen levels are high.20,23 Also, in cases of Turner syndrome (XO), ovarian development occurs normally until the 4th–5th month of intrauterine life after which it prematurely undergoes involution. Inferior performance of these patients in tasks that imply attention and memory, prone to improvement with the administration of estrogens.24,25 In neuroimaging studies during neuropsychological task performance in temporal and parietal cortical areas in young and post-menopausal women receiving estrogen treatment, greater activation, increased blood perfusion and glucose metabolism in certain brain regions related with memory and other cognitive functions have been found.26,27 The results can still not be regarded conclusive due to the reduced number of studies and the differences in methods used.

However, in our opinion, the explanation could also be linked to the way the female child grows up. Usually, they sit quiet, are calm and observe things keenly. This learning due to intense observational ability influences learning and memory in women. The result of higher scores for language and attention in females, although statistically insignificant, can also be explained on the basis of the reason given above. It is generally believed that the average 20-month old young girl has twice the vocabulary skills of the average same age old boy. It is also reported that Toddler girls begin to talk sooner and more clearly than boys.28 Therefore, males are much more likely to be diagnosed with stuttering and problems with handwriting.29,30

On the other hand, in our study, males outshine females with statistically significant results in the mathematical ability akin to prior studies.4,5 The reason speculated by Gur et al based on fMRI showed increased white matter in males contributes towards their analytical skills and transfer of information to different regions of brain thereby, helpful in problem solving capacity.6

Conclusion

This study provides preliminary data to show that men are not intellectually superior to women in visuospatial skills despite bigger skull size and additional studies should be done in this direction. The handling of computers, driving, participation in outdoor activities since early childhood and rising education and job opportunities for girls have brought drastic advances in the skills. These results can further be highlighted in view of the educational status of the subjects (medical students) of the present study representing changes in lifestyles.

Footnotes

The article complies with International Committee of Medical Journal Editor’s uniform requirements for the manuscripts.

Competing interests: None

Source of funding: None

References

1. Huppert F, Wilcock G. Ageing, cognition and dementia. Age and Aging. 1997;26–S4:20–23. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
2. Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bedirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, Cummings J L et al. lo The Montreal Cogntive Assessment, MoCA: A brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2005;53(4):695–699. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
3. Kimura D. Sex, sexual orientation and sex hormones influence human cognitive function. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 1996;6:259–263. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
4. Herlitz A, Loven J. Sex differences in cognitive functions. Acta Psychologica Sinica. 2009;41(11):1081–1090. [Google Scholar]
5. Downing K, Chan SW. Measuring gender differences in cognitive functioning. Multicultural Education & Technology Journal. 2008;2(1):418–432. [Google Scholar]
6. Halpern DF. A cognitive-process taxonomy for sex differences in cognitive abilities. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2004;13(4):135–139. [Google Scholar]
7. Voyer E, Voyer S, Bryden MP. Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: A metaanalysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychological Bulletin. 1995;117:250–270. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
8. Drubach D. Upper saddle river, NJ: Prentice Hall 1e; 2000. The brain explained. p. 188. [Google Scholar]
9. Gur RC, Turetsky BI, Matsui M et al. Sex differences in brain gray and white matter in healthy young adults: Correlations with cognitive performance. Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;19(10):4065–4072. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
10. Sowell ER, Peterson BS, Kan E et al. Sex differences in cortical thickness mapped in 176 healthy individuals between 7 & 87 years of age. Cerebral Cortex. 2007;17:1550–1560. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
11. Bosco A, Longoni AM, Vecchi T. Gender effects in spatial orientation: cognitive profiles and mental st rategies. Applied Cognitive Psychology. 2004;18(5):519–32. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
12. Mann VA, Sasanuma S, Sakuma N, Masaki S. Sex Differences in Cognitive Abilities: A Cross-Cultural Perspective. Neuropsychologia. 1990;28:1063–1077. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
13. Janowsky JS. Thinking with your gonads: Testosterone and cognition. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2006;10:77–82. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
14. Silverman I, Kastuk D, Choi J, Phillips K. Testosterone levels and spatial ability in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1999;24:813–822. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
15. Moffat SD, Hampson E. A curvilinear relationship between testosterone and spatial cognition in humans: Possible influence of hand preference. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1996;21:323–337. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
16. Christiansen K, Knussmann R. Sex hormones and cognitive functioning in men. Neuropsychobiology. 1987;18:27–36. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
17. Gouchie C, Kimura D. The relationship between testosterone levels and cognitive ability patterns. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1991;16:323–334. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
18. Hassler M, Gupta D, Wollmann H. Testosterone, estradiol, ACTH and musical, spatial and verbal performance. International Journal of Neuroscience. 1992;65:45–60. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
19. Berenbaum SA. Effects of early androgens on Sex-Typed activities and Interests in adolescents with Congenital Adrenal Hyperplasia. Hormones and Behavior. 1998;35:102–110. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
20. Hausmann M, Slabbekoorn D, Van Goozen SHM, Cohen KPT, Güntürkün O. Sex hormones affect spatial abilities during the menstrual cycle. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2000;114:1245–1250. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
21. Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. Psychological Corporation; San Antonio: 1987. California Verbal Learning Test. Research edition. Adult version Manual. [Google Scholar]
22. Chipman K, Kimura D. An Investigation of Sex Differences on Incidental Memory for Verbal and Pictorial Material. Learning and Individual Differences. 1998;10:259–272. [Google Scholar]
23. Saucier DM, Kimura D. Intrapersonal but not extrapersonal targeting skills is enhanced during the midluteal phase of the mestrual cycle. Developmental Neuropsychology. 1998;14:385–398. [Google Scholar]
24. Torres A, Gómez-Gil A, Vidal O et al. Gender differences in cognitive functions and influence of sex hormones. Actas Esp Psiquiatr. 2006;34(6):408–415. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
25. Ross JL, Stefanatos G, Roeltgen D et al. Ullrich-Turner syndrome: neurodevelopmental changes from childhood through adolescence. Am J Med Gen. 1995;57:1–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
26. Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA, Pugh KR et al. Effect of estrogen on brain activation patterns in post-menopausal women during working memory tasks. JAMA. 1999;281:1197–202. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
27. Greene RA. Estrogen and cerebral blood flow: a mechanism to explain the impact of estrogen on the incidence and treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. Int J Fertil Menopausal Stud. 2000;45:253–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
28. Kimura D. Cambridge, MA: A Bradford Book/The MIT Press; 2000. Sex and Cognition. [Google Scholar]
29. Howell P, Davis S, Williams R. Late childhood stuttering. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research. 2008;51(3):669–687. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
30. Berninger VW, Nielsen KH, Abbott RD et al. Gender differences in severity of writing and reading disabilities. Journal of School Psychology. 2008;46:151–172. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Annals of Neurosciences are provided here courtesy of SAGE Publications

-